Skip to main content

Effects of craving and DRD4 VNTR genotype on the relative value of alcohol: an initial human laboratory study

Abstract

Background

Craving for alcohol is a highly controversial subjective construct and may be clarified by Loewenstein's visceral theory, which emphasizes craving's behavioral effects on the relative value of alcohol. Based on the visceral theory, this study examined the effects of a craving induction on the relative value of alcohol as measured by a behavioral choice task. In addition, based on previous evidence of its role in the expression of craving, the influence of DRD4 VNTR genotype (DRD4-L vs. DRD4-S) was also examined.

Methods

Thirty-five heavy drinkers (54% male; 31% DRD4-L) were randomly assigned to receive either a craving induction (exposure to personally relevant alcohol cues) or a control induction (exposure to neutral cues), which was followed by an alcohol-money choice task. Participants were assessed for craving and positive/negative affect throughout the procedure, and relative value of alcohol was derived from participant choices for alcohol versus money. DRD4 VNTR status was assessed retrospectively via buccal samples using previously established protocols.

Results

Factorial analysis of the craving induction revealed that it was associated with significant increase in craving (p < .001), but not greater relative value of alcohol. Factorial analyses including DRD4 VNTR genotype of did not suggest an influence on reactivity to the craving induction, although this analysis was substantially compromised by small cell sample sizes. Continuous analyses revealed that craving was significantly associated with the relative value of alcohol (p < .05) and possession of the DRD4-L allele further amplified this relationship (p < .001).

Conclusion

These results are interpreted as generally supporting Loewenstein's visceral theory of craving and evidence of a functional role of DRD4 VNTR genotype in the expression of craving for alcohol. Methodological limitations, mechanisms underlying these findings, and future directions are discussed.

Background

Despite the long history of research on craving for alcohol [e.g., [1]], the functional relationship between craving and both alcohol use and post-treatment relapse remains highly contentious [2, 3]. This controversy is, in part, a result of considerable ambiguity in the data connecting craving and alcohol misuse. Although alcohol dependent individuals readily report experiencing cravings for alcohol [4, 5], the associations between self-reported craving and actual alcohol use in human laboratory studies have been equivocal [for a review, see [6]; cf. [7, 8]]. Similarly, in clinical research, a number of retrospective studies have reported a negligible role of craving in relapse [9–11]. Given the ambiguous empirical data, it is not surprising that there is little consensus as to the role of craving in alcohol use disorders [2, 12].

The present study sought to better understand craving for alcohol using a novel theoretical approach, Loewenstein's [13, 14] visceral theory of addiction. The visceral theory is a hybrid theory that integrates decision theory [e.g., [15, 16]] and learning theory [e.g., [17, 18]] via a common pathway of behavioral economics. From this perspective, addiction is an extreme example of a wide array of behaviors that are largely controlled by visceral factors, or drive states such as hunger, thirst, sexual desire, or, in the case of addiction, craving. The decision to use a substance is considered to be the final determinant of substance use, which is itself guided by the value of the substance relative to all other available choices (i.e., relative value). However, the relative value of a substance is not a static property and it is proposed to be substantially influenced by a number of variables, most prominently, craving. Specifically, the visceral theory proposes that it is not simply the experience of craving that precipitates substance use, but rather it is the effects of craving, including increased relative value of the substance, increased discounting of delayed rewards, and narrowed temporal perception, among others. Over time, substance misuse is proposed to be a function of these effects recurrently biasing an individual's behavior toward continued substance use over more salubrious options. Importantly, by virtue of its emphasis on craving's effects on the value of the substance, Loewenstein's visceral theory is distinct from other contemporary theories of craving that emphasize craving as a classically conditioned response [18–20], an affective process [21, 22], or a cognitive process [23]. In addition, the visceral theory can be clearly distinguished from approaches that propose craving to primarily reflect the interruption of automatized addictive behaviour, and, as a result, to be largely epiphenomenological [6, 24].

Although the visceral theory has not been subjected to substantial direct empirical testing, a number of its predictions have been borne out by the empirical literature [e.g., [25–27]]. This includes one of the visceral theory's central predictions, that craving increases the relative value of a substance, which has received provisional empirical support in a number of studies. These studies have largely been in reference to tobacco dependence, where evidence that craving can be understood in measures of economic value has been obliquely supported by several laboratory studies using craving-related manipulations (e.g., enforced abstinence) that resulted in increases in the relative value of smoking [28–30]. However, because these studies were not intended to directly test the visceral account, the majority lack critical variables, such as the subjects' level of craving, or did not report the relationship between craving and the relative value of smoking.

More convincingly, three studies have directly examined and reported the relationship between craving and the relative value of smoking with generally positive results. In an initial study, Willner et al. [31] found a significant increase in the relative value of tobacco following deprivation using a progressive-ratio operant task. Similarly, Perkins et al. [32] found a positive association between craving and subjects' perceived cash value of tobacco, although craving was not associated with effort expended on an operant task. Finally, Sayette et al. [33] conducted a multi-dimensional assessment of cue-elicited craving and found a significant association between craving and the relative value of smoking as measured using a forced-choice self-administration task.

Comparatively little research from this perspective has been conducted in reference to craving for alcohol, with only one study in that domain. In the context of a laboratory study on the mechanisms of naltrexone pharmacotherapy, O'Malley et al. [7] administered a priming dose of alcohol and then used a self-administration paradigm that permitted participants to "buy" drinks. Post-priming subjective craving was found to significantly correlate with the number of alcoholic beverages chosen (and money foregone), thus converging with the majority of evidence from tobacco research that subjective craving can be understood in terms of relative value. Considered together, the preceding studies on craving for tobacco and alcohol suggest that the visceral theory is a promising approach for understanding craving. To further test the theory, the first goal of the current study was to examine the effects of a craving induction on the relative value of alcohol.

The second goal of the current study sought to examine a potential neurogenetic variable that may influence the relationship between craving and the relative value of alcohol. There is considerable evidence that one of the major neural substrates of craving is mesolimbic dopaminergic neurotransmission [34, 35], although not exclusively [e.g., [36]]. Moreover, there is recent evidence that alleles of the gene responsible for the dopamine D4 receptor, which is localized within the limbic system, may play an important role in the expression of craving for alcohol [37–40]. Specifically, the D4 receptor gene (DRD4) has a variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) polymorphism in exon 3, with common variants of 2, 4, and 7 repeats [41], and there is evidence that possession of a long allele (7 repeats or more; DRD4-L) confers functional differences in dopamine neurotransmission [42–44]. Vis-à-vis craving, previous studies have demonstrated that DRD4-L status positively moderates the effects of both an alcohol cue exposure [37, 40] and priming dose of alcohol on craving [38]. Similar interactions between DRD4 VNTR genotype and craving have also been found in laboratory studies of smoking [45], heroin addiction [46], and binge eating [47]. Although the exact role of the DRD4 VNTR genotype remains unclear, these studies suggest that possession of a long version of the DRD4 VNTR genotype may also enhance the effect of craving on the relative value of alcohol.

Thus, this study was a preliminary investigation using the visceral theory as a framework for concurrently examining the influences of a craving induction and DRD4 VNTR genotype on the relative value of alcohol. Heavy drinkers were enrolled in a laboratory protocol and randomized to receive either a craving induction (i.e., exposure to personally relevant alcohol cues) or a control induction (i.e., exposure to neutral cues), followed by an alcohol-money choice task to assess the relative value of alcohol. A cue exposure approach was employed as the craving induction because it has been robustly validated for inducing craving in the laboratory [for a review, see [48]], and has the advantage of not including the array of concomitant physiological effects of induced withdrawal [e.g., [30]] or mild intoxication [7]. Moreover, the visceral theory proposes that the most important form of craving is cue-elicited craving that persists beyond treatment [13, 14]. Based on the visceral theory, the craving induction was predicted to be associated with greater relative value of alcohol compared to the control induction, and the absolute level of craving independent of induction was predicted to be significantly associated with relative value of alcohol. Although the existing literature on DRD4 VNTR effects is relatively small, DRD4-L status was predicted to both positively moderate the effects of the craving induction, replicating two previous studies [37, 40], and the continuous relationship between craving and the relative value of alcohol. Based on theoretical interest in the relationship between craving and affect [21, 22, 49], positive and negative affect was concurrently assessed throughout the study, but given ambiguous associations in previous studies [e.g., [49]], no directional predictions were made.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited using posted advertisements and classroom/email solicitations at the State University of New York at Binghamton. They were required to be heavy drinkers, defined as drinking at least 20+/14+ standard drinks per week for males/females [50] and of legal age for alcohol consumption (21 years old in New York State). Since the multimodal craving induction was oriented around beer, prospective participants were required to report beer as both a favorite and most often consumed alcoholic beverage, and to rate their enjoyment of beer seven or greater on a 10-point Likert-type scale. Participants were compensated with $10, plus any money selected during the choice task.

Thirty five (54% male; mean age = 21.83 [SD = 1.12]) heavy drinkers were enrolled in the study. Male participants drank an average of 36.32 (SD = 12.98) drinks/week and female participants drank an average of 21.00 drinks/week (SD = 6.54). These levels reflect the 97th and 98th percentiles of alcohol consumption for this cohort [51]. The mean AUDIT score was 14.2 (SD = 5.58) and 100% of participants scored an 8 or higher, the previously validated criterion for hazardous drinking [52]. Eleven (31%) participants were classified as DRD4-L based on possession of at least one version of the DRD4 VNTR allele with seven or more repeats. Median household income was $100,000 (IQR = $65,000–$135,000). In terms of race/ethnicity, 83% of the sample was Caucasian, 14% of the sample was Hispanic, and 3% of the sample was Asian.

Experimental design

The study used a one-way two-group between-subjects design. Upon enrollment, participants were randomly assigned to receive either the craving induction or control induction and all underwent the relative value of alcohol task immediately following the post-induction assessment. In considering DRD4 VNTR influences, the design was elaborated to 2 (craving induction/control induction) × 2 (DRD4-L/DRD4-S) quasi-experimental factorial design. The latter was quasi-experimental because subjects cannot be randomly assigned to genotype. In addition to the factorial design, based on the visceral theory's emphasis on the importance of the absolute level of craving [13, 14], continuous relationships were also examined and are described further in the Data Analysis section.

Measures

Drinking days questionnaire (DDQ)

The DDQ is a seven-item, face-valid measure of an individual's average alcohol consumption per week. Its seven items assess the typical amount consumed for each day of the week and it has been shown to have adequate psychometric properties [53, 54].

Alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT)

The AUDIT [52] is a ten-item self-report screening measure that evaluates various quantitative and symptom-related aspects of drinking. The AUDIT yields a score from 0–40; a score of 8 or above indicates hazardous drinking, or drinking at risk for negative psychosocial consequences. The AUDIT has been extensively validated in terms of psychometric properties with high sensitivity and specificity [52], and had a coefficient α of .74 in this sample.

Subjective craving

A single item measure of craving [e.g., [33, 35]] was employed in this study and followed recommendations for conceptualizing craving on a continuum of urges for alcohol [56, 57]. In this case, a 100-point Likert-type scale of urge to drink was used with four anchoring comments ("I don't want a beer at all," "I don't want a beer very much," "I'd like a beer now," and "I'd REALLY like a beer right now!!!") to enhance the likelihood of similar interpretation of the scale by participants. The first and last comments were placed at the low and high ends of the scale, respectively, and the two intermediate comments were beneath the numbers 35 and 70, respectively.

Positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS)

Positive and negative affect were assessed using the PANAS [58]. The PANAS is a 20-item measure of transient mood that has undergone extensive previous psychometric validation [58, 59]. In this sample, under neutral conditions, the positive affect subscale of the PANAS generated a baseline coefficient α of .87 and the negative affect subscale generated a baseline coefficient α of .85.

Relative value of alcohol task

The relative value of alcohol task was based on previous methods used to assess relative value in behavioral economic research [29, 60, 61]. Because craving states are relatively transient [62, 63] and delays of rewards substantially influence their relative value [60], the task was designed to be as short as possible (~90 seconds) and all alcohol and/or money selected was provided immediately following the task. To maximize the validity of the choice task, participants were provided with actual alcohol and/or money based on their choices.

Procedurally, each participant's relative value of alcohol was empirically defined in monetary terms using a series of choices to determine the average amount of money a participant would "pay" for a sip of alcohol. Participants completed nine choices between a 1.5-ounce sip of the participant's favorite beer (alcohol) and varying amounts of money to determine this point. This sip volume was selected because the total volume of alcohol available was one standard beer and 1.5 ozs represented approximately one ninth of the beer, which was an appropriate size for a sip. In addition, this unit was easily conveyable to participants. The first choice was between a sip of alcohol and $1, and the overall domain of potential choices and increments was as follows: $.01, $.02, $.05, $.10, $.25, $.50, $1 (starting point), $1.50, $2, $2.50, $3, $3.50, $4, $4.50, and $5.

The task used an adaptive adjusting procedure based on the participant's responses over the course of the nine choices. If the participant chose alcohol, the amount of money offered on the next choice was increased by one increment; if the participant chose money, the amount of money offered on the next trial decreased by one increment. Relative value of alcohol was defined as the value that reflected the individual's change in preference from alcohol to money and vice versa [i.e., "crossover" point; [29]]. Specifically, relative value of alcohol was defined as the mean of the smallest amount of money chosen over a sip of alcohol (inferring that alcohol was worth less than that amount) and the largest amount of money over which alcohol was chosen (inferring that alcohol was worth more than that amount). Of note, the critical dependent variable in this study was relative value of alcohol, not the absolute amount of money earned in the task. As a result of the adjusting procedure in the task, there was not a linear relationship between the number of choices made for money and the amount of money earned. Equally, since the adaptive adjusting procedure was responsive to choice, the absolute numbers of choices for money or alcohol were redundant with relative value of alcohol and not used as dependent variables.

Genotyping

A sample of buccal cells was collected from each participant using an oral swab, following previously published procedures [37]. Participants swabbed their cheeks with three cotton swabs and then rinsed their mouths with 10 ml distilled water. The swabs and wash were placed in a sterile 50 ml polypropylene tube containing 500 μl of 1 M Tris-EDTA buffer (1 M Tris-HCl, 200 μM disodium EDTA, pH 8.0), 500 μl of 5% sodium dodecyl sulfate (SDS), and 100 μl of 5 M sodium chloride. The tubes were refrigerated until DNA was extracted. To extract the DNA, proteinase K (0.2 mg/ml) was added to the tubes and the tubes were incubated at 65°C for 60 min. The swabs were removed and residual lysis buffer was extracted by centrifugation (using a 3-ml syringe barrel and sterile 15 ml tube) for 5 minutes at 1,000 × g. The residual fluid was added back to the original sample. An equal volume of isopropyl alcohol was then added to each tube, the contents were mixed, and the DNA was collected by centrifugation at 3,500 × g for 10 min. The DNA pellet was rinsed once with 1 ml of 50% isopropyl alcohol and allowed to air dry. The pellet was resuspended in 1 ml of 10 μM TRIS-HCl, 10 mM EDTA buffer (pH 8.0) and place in a 1.8-ml cryovial. The concentration of DNA was calculated from the absorbance at 260 nm. Samples were stored at -70°C until used. The collection and extraction rate was 100%; all participants were retrospectively genotyped.

The 48 basepair VNTR in exon 3 of the DRD4 gene was assayed using primer sequences: forward, 5'-AGGACCCTCATGGCCT TG-3' (fluorescently labeled), and reverse 5'-CGACTACGTGGTCTACTCG-3' [64]. Genotyping success rate was 100%. Consistent with previous research [37–40], participants who were homozygous or heterozygous for an allele of seven repeats or longer were classified as DRD4-L and all other participants were classified as DRD4-S. The allele frequencies of the sample are presented in Table 1.

Table 1 DRD4 VNTR allele and genotype frequencies; percentages do not always add to 100 due to rounding error.

Procedure

All study procedures were approved by the Human Subjects Research Review Committee at the State University of New York at Binghamton. The experiment involved one laboratory session that took place in the afternoon and lasted 90-minutes. Participants were informed that the session would last the full duration to avoid any perceived contingency between not drinking and early departure, and that they would be compensated $10 but could also potentially receive additional money. In addition, participants were informed that they would have access to alcohol [65] and would be required to endorse that there were no significant reasons why they could not drink if they chose to during the session (e.g., medication, athletic event, examination). In advance of the experimental session, participants were sequentially randomized to receive the craving or control induction.

During the experimental session, participants initially provided informed consent in a neutral experimental room and were assessed for breath alcohol. If any breath alcohol was detected, the session would be terminated and they would be rescheduled; no participants required rescheduling. Participants completed the alcohol use, craving, and affect measures. They were then escorted to either the alcohol or neutral cue exposure room and underwent the craving induction or control induction.

The craving induction consisted of exposure to an array of personally relevant alcohol cues, including visual, olfactory, tactile, imaginal, and proprioceptive cues. Participants were introduced into a dimmed laboratory room (8 ft × 6 ft × 8 ft [2.44 m × 1.83 m × 2.44 m]) decorated with beer-related paraphernalia (posters and advertisements of beer, barroom trifolds, empty beer bottles). A Research Assistant (RA) then brought a bottle of the participant's favorite beer and poured it into a beer glass. Participants were then left alone for 60 seconds to observe the array of cues. After one minute, the RA returned and informed the participants that they would be asked to listen to an imaginal scene. In addition, participants were told that during the scene they would be asked to smell the beer from time to time. To establish the correct behavior, participants were then asked to lift the beer to their nose and take five inhalations of the smell of the beer. The RA then provided instructions for listening to the scene and left the room, observing via a one-way window that the participant correctly followed the scene-related instructions. Over the course of the scene, participants were asked to hold the beer up to their nose and deeply inhale the smell of the beer for five seconds on five occasions (total olfactory exposure with practice = 30 seconds). The imaginal scenes were developed based on previous research on imaginal scenes in cue reactivity research [66, 67] and described common environmental, interpersonal, and affective contexts of drinking, as well as evocative descriptions of the orosensory properties of beer. Participants were matched to their favorite brand of beer [63] and were also matched to the imaginal scenes that related to their most common reason for drinking from among seven possibilities: relaxation, happiness, enjoyment of the taste, anger, boredom, sadness, anxiety, or habit. Favorite beer and appropriate imaginal scene had been assessed during screening; 32 participants received the happy scene, 2 received the bored scene, and 1 received the sad scene. The control induction was matched in each respect but took place in a different room and all cues, including imaginal scene, were oriented around consuming water. For both groups, the cue exposure periods were equivalent (~8 min).

Following the cue exposure, participants completed the craving and affect measures a second time, and then completed the relative value of alcohol task. Participants were informed that they now had the opportunity to choose either a 1.5 oz sip of their favorite beer or various amounts of money. The volume of the sip was demonstrated using water in a glass. In addition, the participants were informed that as soon as the task was over, they would be provided with the total amount of alcohol and/or money that they chose. The provision of the alcohol/money concluded the experimental portion of the procedure. Participants then returned to an undecorated experimental room (without either alcohol or water cues) and underwent the genotyping procedure, before relaxing in that room for the remainder of the session (approximately one hour). This period was used to ensure that no participants would leave the study intoxicated. At the conclusion of the session, a debriefing was conducted and breath alcohol was established at 0.00%. All genotyping was conducted retrospectively, following the completion of enrollment.

Data analysis

All baseline data were examined for outlying data points and distribution normality. All participants were examined on alcohol use and other self-report variables to evaluate potential baseline differences as a result of induction assignment or DRD4 VNTR genotype. Principal analyses used hierarchical multiple regression to examine the influence of covariates, induction type, genotype, and induction-by-genotype interaction; the latter term was examined for evidence of a moderating genetic influence [68]. Following the factorial analyses, continuous analyses were conducted between the experiential variables and the relative value of alcohol. The difference between the factorial and continuous analyses was that the former used the induction type (i.e., craving versus control induction) as an independent variable, whereas the latter used the participants' actual values for the experiential variables (i.e., craving and affect) as an independent variable. Both factorial and continuous approaches were employed because Loewenstein's [13, 14] visceral theory proposes that the absolute level of craving is the most significant determinant of the relative value of alcohol and a continuous analysis addressed this more directly. Specific analytic strategies for each portion of the study are provided in the Results section. In all cases, statistical significance was determined by examining the initial regression model fit (R2, F-ratio) and relative improvement of the model fit (ΔR2, F-ratio) in subsequent blocks. Where multiple variables were included in a multiple regression block, each variable was evaluated by examining the significance of the variable coefficient. Follow-up analyses were conducted to examine the possibility of population stratification [69], a "third variable" confound based on differential associations between a given phenotype and racial/ethnic differences. For all significant genetic findings, population stratification was addressed by re-examining the findings within the largest single racial group (a group with minimized racial admixture).

Results

Baseline analyses

All baseline dependent variables were adequately normally distributed. No data were missing and no outliers were identified. Using one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs), no significant baseline differences were evident between participants randomized to the two inductions on the following variables: drinks/week, AUDIT score, craving, positive affect, and negative affect (all p s > .20). One-way ANOVAs also revealed no significant differences between DRD4-L and DRD4-S genotypes on the following variables: drinks/week, AUDIT score, craving, positive affect, and negative affect (all p s > .20). Because of the potential relevance of income, drinks/week, and level of problems with alcohol (i.e., AUDIT score) to the dependent variables, these variables were also initially examined as potential covariates using simple regressions. Coefficient estimates revealed that income, drinks/week, and AUDIT were not significantly associated with post-induction craving (p s > .10), relative value of alcohol (ps > .20), and positive affect (p s > .20). However, drinks/week was significantly negatively associated with post-cue exposure negative affect (β = -.52, p < .01) and was included as a covariate in the subsequent negative affect analyses; income and AUDIT score were nonsignificantly associated with post-induction negative affect (p s > .14).

Influences of the craving induction and subjective craving on the relative value of alcohol

Induction effects were examined using hierarchical multiple regression to sequentially examine the influence of covariates and the effects of the experimental manipulation, with the post-induction values of a variable serving as the dependent variable, its pre-induction level entered as a covariate in a first block (with the exception of relative value of alcohol, which had no baseline level), and induction type entered into a second block. For the effects of the craving induction, the covariate model was significant (R2 = .54; F (1, 33) = 38.35, p < .001), indicating that baseline craving was significantly associated with post-induction craving. The addition of the induction type in a second model significantly improved the model (ΔR2 = .17, F (1, 32) = 18.52, p < .001) and reflected a significant increase in craving for individuals receiving the craving induction compared to the control induction, as depicted in Figure 1. For positive affect, the covariate model was significant (R2 = .88, F (1, 33) = 261.79, p < .001), but the addition of induction type did not significantly improve the model (ΔR2 = .00, F (1, 32) = .05, p > .80). For negative affect, the covariate model including both drinks/week and negative affect was significant (R2 = .77, F (2, 32) = 54.80, p < .001), but the addition of induction type did not significantly improve the model (ΔR2 = .00, F (1, 31) = .23, p > .60). Coefficient estimates for the model revealed a significant association with baseline negative affect (p < .001) and a marginally significant association of drinks/week (p = .10). In terms of the effect of the craving induction on the relative value of alcohol, no significant effect was evident (R2 = .01, F (1, 33) = .02, p > .80).

Figure 1
figure 1

Effects of the craving induction (personalized alcohol cues) and control induction (neutral cues) on urge (i.e., craving) for alcohol; *** p < .001.

Following the factorial analyses (i.e., induction condition), continuous analyses were conducted between the experiential variables (i.e., craving and affect) and the relative value of alcohol using the same regression-based approach, but with post-induction variable values serving as the independent variable. Continuous examination of the relationship between post-cue exposure craving and the relative value of alcohol revealed a significant association (R2 = .15, F (1, 33) = 5.711, β = .38, p < .05), reflecting a positive relationship between craving and relative value of alcohol, as depicted in Figure 2. Neither positive (R2 = .00, F (1, 33) = .00, p > .90), nor negative affect (R2 = .00, F (1, 33) = .00, p > .90) were significantly associated with relative value of alcohol.

Figure 2
figure 2

Continuous association between urge (i.e., craving) for alcohol and the relative value of alcohol from an alcohol-money choice task; β = .38, p < .05.

Influences of DRD4 VNTR genotype

To examine the influence of DRD4 VNTR genotype, DRD4 VNTR status and the induction-by-genotype interaction term were added to the preceding hierarchical multiple regression analyses of induction effects. Specifically, the post-induction value of a variable served as the dependent variable, its pre-induction level was entered as a covariate in a first block (with the exception of relative value of alcohol, which had no baseline level), induction type was entered in a second block, and genotype and the interaction term were entered in a third block; DRD4 VNTR status was coded as DRD4-S = 1 and DRD4-L = 2. Frequencies of genotypes revealed the following cell sample sizes for the factorial analyses: craving induction-DRD4-S = 10; craving induction-DRD4-L = 8; control induction-DRD4-S = 14; control induction-DRD4-L = 3. Given the preceding cell sizes, particularly the latter, the 2 × 2 factorial analyses had very limited power but were nonetheless conducted. In terms of craving, the addition of genotype and induction-by-genotype interaction term did not significantly improve the model (ΔR2 = .01, F (2, 30) = .39, p > .65), as was the case for relative value of alcohol (ΔR2 = .08, F (2, 31) = 1.24, p > .40), positive affect (ΔR2 = .00, F (2, 30) = .10, p > .80) or negative affect (F (2, 29) = .09, p > .80).

The issue of small cell sample size did not apply to the continuous analyses and, in the case of craving, the addition of DRD4 VNTR status and the interaction term resulted in a significant improvement of the model (ΔR2 = .22, F (1, 33) = 5.22, p = .01). Variable coefficients are provided in Table 2 and revealed that this improvement was accounted for by a significant association for the craving-by-genotype interaction. The interaction effect is depicted in Figure 3 and reflected a relationship such that the influence of craving on the relative value of alcohol was disproportionately high for DRD4-L individuals. No significant independent effects or moderating effects of DRD4 VNTR status were evident for positive (ΔR2 = .13, F (2, 31) = 2.27, p > .10) or negative affect (ΔR2 = .09, F (2, 31) = 1.50, p > .20).

Table 2 Associations between craving, DRD4 VNTR genotype, and the interaction with the relative value of alcohol.
Figure 3
figure 3

Association between the craving by DRD4 VNTR genotype interaction term and the relative value of alcohol (β = 1.70, p < .01).

Population stratification

The likelihood of population stratification was considered low because there is no evidence to date of population stratification for the phenotypes under consideration (i.e., craving, relative value of alcohol, affect). In addition, the overall sample in this study was relatively small and the large majority of participants were of the same race (Caucasian); proportions of participants by genotype and by race are shown in Table 3. Nonetheless, to address the possibility of population stratification, the significant genetic findings were re-conducted using only the Caucasian participants, which revealed the same pattern of findings as the principal analyses. The preceding combined model (i.e., craving, genotype, craving-by-genotype interaction) was significant (p < .001), neither craving, nor DRD4 VNTR coefficients were independently significant (p s > .30), and the interaction coefficient was significant (p < .05), reflecting the same positive relationship. Formal tests of population stratification using genomic control were not conducted.

Table 3 Genotype by race/ethnicity.

Discussion

This preliminary examination of the influences of craving for alcohol and DRD4 VNTR genotype on the relative value of alcohol generated mixed results. As anticipated, the craving induction resulted in a significant increase in subjective craving compared to the control induction, but contrary to predictions, it was not associated with greater relative value of alcohol. However, when the data were considered continuously, independent of experimental condition, craving was significantly associated with the relative value of alcohol. Parallel findings were evident when considering the influence of DRD4 VNTR genotype. DRD4-L status did not significantly moderate the effects of the alcohol cue exposure, but when considered continuously, DRD4 VNTR genotype significantly moderated the relationship between craving and the relative value of alcohol. The form of this relationship was such that as craving increased, DRD4-L individuals exhibited a disproportionately greater relative value of alcohol. Across the procedures, affect was not influenced by the craving induction and was not associated with the relative value of alcohol. To most clearly discuss these findings, we will first consider the specific findings relating to the craving induction and will then consider the role of DRD4 VNTR genotype.

Effects of the craving induction

With regard to the effects of the craving induction compared to the control induction, the discrepancy between the factorial and continuous findings provides only mixed support for the visceral theory. On one hand, the lack of effect of the alcohol cue exposure is in direct contrast to what the visceral theory would predict, but, on the other hand, the significant continuous association between craving and relative value is consistent with the visceral theory's prediction that the absolute level of craving determines the relative value of a commodity. Although these findings appear to be contradictory, they may be understood in the context of the cue exposure craving induction itself. Although the cue exposure paradigm typically generates significant increases in craving in aggregate, there is considerable variation in participants' reactions and in the absolute levels of craving that are reported [48, 49]. This is compounded by the fact that participants report a wide array of levels of craving in general prior to the procedure, which contributes to the overall heterogeneity. For example, in the current study, craving at baseline ranged from 3 to 79 out of 100. Thus, although subjects receiving the alcohol cue exposure may have reported greater craving in aggregate, it appears that was still sufficient heterogeneity between the two groups for that effect to not translate into a significant effect on relative value.

In addition, some additional methodological considerations may be relevant to no significant effect of the craving induction on the relative value of alcohol. First, although the relative value task was intentionally designed to be short and provide immediate tangible rewards, it could only capture one facet of relative value, which is itself a multidimensional construct [70, 71]. Second, the relative value of alcohol task only used a maximum of one standard alcoholic beverage, which would have relatively limited psychoactive effects and may have affected participant's valuation of the beverage. Third, the task was administered on only one occasion because it directly provided alcohol which would have affected subsequent performance. However, as a result, it could not be administered on multiple occasions and thus did not provide a baseline relative value of alcohol. Each of these aspects of the methodology could have contributed to the lack of effect in the factorial analyses and should be considered in future studies.

Despite these potential explanations for the factorial findings, performance on the relative value of alcohol task was nonetheless sensitive to variation in craving in the continuous analyses, which accords with the prediction of the visceral theory. Indeed, a cornerstone of the theory is that the absolute level of craving is a critical determinant of behaviour, which is also supported by the current data where overall experiential craving was more important than the experimental manipulation.

Influences of DRD4 VNTR genotype

Like the effects of the craving induction, the influence of DRD4 VNTR genotype was mixed, exhibiting a negligible influence in the factorial analyses but a substantial influence in the continuous analyses. Although potential methodological explanations for this discrepancy are the same as those described in the preceding section, it is also important to note that the study was relatively small in general and retrospective genotyping resulted in very few subjects in one cell in the induction-by-genotype factorial analyses. As a result, the factorial genetic analyses had very low statistical power, substantially limiting the interpretability of those findings. Importantly, rather than interpreting the lack of moderating effect of DRD4 VNTR genotype on reaction to alcohol cues as diverging from previous findings [37, 40], it appears more prudent to conclude that this study could not fully test that hypothesis.

In contrast, in the continuous analyses where power was not limited by cell size, the data conformed to predictions. As self-reported craving increased, those individuals possessing the long variant exhibited greater valuation of alcohol on the behavioral task. These data are broadly consistent with previous studies that have suggested that DRD4-L status is associated with amplified expression of craving for alcohol [37–40], as well as other appetitive targets, such as cigarettes [45], heroin [46], and food [47]. Of note, although the threat of population stratification is low in general [69] and was considered to be low in this study, this was confirmed by replicating the principal findings in the subsample reporting Caucasian ancestry.

From a mechanistic standpoint, these findings are consistent with Hutchison et al.'s [45] proposal that possession of a DRD4-L allelic variant enhances an individual's sensitivity to dopaminergic rewards, including alcohol use. Although characterizing the functional significance of polymorphisms of the DRD4 VNTR genotype remains an active basic research question [72], Hutchison et al. [45] propose that this influence may be via functional differences between the polymorphisms in accumulation of cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) in response to dopamine agonism. D4 receptors are members of the D2 receptor family and, as such, inhibit cAMP formation, however, the long DRD4 VNTR variant permits two to three times greater accumulation of cAMP [32]. As a result, DRD4-L individuals may be inferred to have chronically elevated levels of cAMP, which, in conjunction with elevations of additional downstream transcriptional factors, has been associated with enhanced sensitivity to dopaminergic rewards in a number of studies using animal models [73–75]. Moreover, such effects are specific to increased in cAMP in the nucleus accumbens, which is both a structure where D4 receptors are localized [76] and one of the key putative substrates of reward motivation [34, 35]. However, despite the plausibility of differences in intracellular cAMP being responsible for the observed interaction, because the functional role of polymorphisms of the DRD4 VNTR genotype has not been fully characterized [72] and the literature on DRD4 VNTR influences on craving in humans is relatively small, it is important to underscore that considerable further research is necessary to understand these relationships.

Future directions

Based on this preliminary study, further examinations of the utility of the visceral theory for understanding craving as a motivational factor appear to be warranted. Methodologically, as noted above, subsequent studies would do well to consider behavioural economic methodologies that allow for dynamic point-to-point assessments of relative value that can incorporate multiple facets of value and potentially more meaningful amounts of alcohol. Clearly, larger sample sizes would permit more exhaustive examinations of genetic contributions to craving, both in terms of statistical power and the alleles under consideration. DRD4 VNTR was the single target of the current study based on the existing literature [37, 38, 40] and the small sample size, however, other recent studies have suggested provocative interactions with other alleles. These include positive moderating effects of craving inductions by minor alleles of the OPRM1, DRD2 TaqI A, and SLC6A3 genes [[77, 78]; cf. [37]], although at this point these represent isolated reports. Finally, it is possible that measures of relative value would have utility in neuroimaging research characterizing the neurobiological structures that subserve addictive behaviour. An extensive literature has demonstrated that craving inductions selectively activate the anterior cingulate, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and orbitofrontal cortex [79–84], however, less is known about which of these structures directly influence subsequent choices for a substance. Such an application would be an archetypal example of the newly burgeoning transdisciplinary field of neuroeconomics [85].

Conclusion

This study used Loewenstein's visceral theory of addiction as the basis for examining the influences of a craving induction and DRD4 VNTR genotype on the relative value of alcohol. Factorial analyses revealed a significant effect of the craving induction on craving, but not on the relative value of alcohol and no interaction with DRD4 VNTR genotype. In contrast, continuous analyses revealed the predicted relationships, with craving significantly associated with the relative value of alcohol and DRD4 VNTR moderating the strength of this relationship. Taken together, these findings are interpreted as generally supporting the potential utility of the visceral theory and further demonstrating the relevance of DRD4 VNTR genotype to understanding the expression of craving for alcohol.

References

  1. Jellinek EM, Isbell H, Lundquist G, Tiebout HM, Duchene H, Mardones J, Macleod LD: The "Craving" for alcohol: A symposium by members of the WHO expert committees on mental health and alcohol. J Stud Alc. 1955, 16: 34-67.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Tiffany ST: Potential functions of classical conditioning in drug addiction. Cue Exposure: Theory and Practice. Edited by: Drummond DC, Tiffany STS, Glautier S, Remington R. 1995, Oxford, England: John Wiley and Sons, 47-71.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Tiffany ST, Carter BL: Is craving the source of compulsive drug use?. J Psychopharmacol. 1998, 12: 23-30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Anton RF, Moak DH, Latham PK: The Obsessive-Compulsive Drinking Scale: A self-rated instrument for the quantification of thoughts about alcohol and drinking behavior. Alc Clin Exp Res. 1995, 19: 92-99. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01475.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  5. Bohn MJ, Krahn DD, Staehler BB: Development and initial validation of a measure of drinking urges in abstinent alcoholics. Alc Clin Exp Res. 1995, 19: 600-606. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01554.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Tiffany ST, Conklin CA: A cognitive processing model of alcohol craving and compulsive alcohol use. Addiction. 2000, 95 (Suppl 2): S145-S153. 10.1080/09652140050111717.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. O'Malley SS, Krishnan SS, Farren C, Sinha R, Kreek MJ: Naltrexone decreases craving and alcohol self-administration in alcohol-dependent subjects and activates the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortial axis. Psychopharmacol. 2002, 160: 19-29. 10.1007/s002130100919.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Palfai TP, Monti PM, Ostafin B, Hutchison K: Effects of nicotine deprivation on alcohol-related information processing and drinking behavior. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000, 109: 96-105. 10.1037/0021-843X.109.1.96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Hodgins DC, el-Guebaly N, Armstrong S: Prospective and retrospective reports of mood states before relapse to substance use. J Cons Clin Psychol. 1995, 63: 400-407. 10.1037/0022-006X.63.3.400.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Lowman C, Allen J, Stout RL: Replication and extension of Marlatt's taxonomy of relapse precipitants, overview of procedures and results. The Relapse Research Group. Addiction. 1996, 91: 51-71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Miller NS, Gold MS: Dissociation of "conscious desire" (craving) from relapse in alcohol and cocaine dependence. Ann Clin Psychiat. 1994, 6: 99-106.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Drummond DC: Theories of craving, ancient and modern. Addiction. 2001, 96: 33-46. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961333.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Loewenstein G: Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organiz Behav Hum Dec Proc. 1996, 65: 272-292. 10.1006/obhd.1996.0028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Loewenstein G: A visceral account of addiction. Smoking: Risk, perception, and policy. Edited by: Slovic P. 2001, Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc, 188-215.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Becker GS, Murphy KM: A rational theory of addiction. J Polit Econ. 1988, 96: 675-700. 10.1086/261558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Herrnstein RJ, Prelec D: A theory of addiction. Choice Over Time. Edited by: Elster J, Loewenstein G. 1992, New York, NY: Guilford Press, 331-360.

    Google Scholar 

  17. O'Brien CP, Childress AR, McLellan AT, Ehrmann R: A learning model of addiction. Addictive States. Edited by: O'Brien CP, Jaffe JH. New York, NY: Raven Press

  18. Siegel S, Ramos BMC: Applying laboratory research: Drug anticipation and the treatment of drug addiction. Exp Clin Psychopharm. 2002, 10: 162-183. 10.1037/1064-1297.10.3.162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ludwig AM, Wikler A, Stark LH: The first drink: Psychobiological aspects of craving. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1974, 30: 539-47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Stewart J, de Wit H, Eikelboom R: Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychol Rev. 1984, 91: 251-68. 10.1037/0033-295X.91.2.251.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Baker TB, Morse E, Sherman JE: The motivation to use drugs: A psychobiological analysis of urges. Nebr Symp Motiv. 1986, 34: 257-323.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC: Addiction motivation reformulated: An affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 2004, 111: 33-51. 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Kavanagh DJ, Andrade J, May J: Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: The Elaborated Intrusion theory of desire. Psychol Rev. 112: 446-467. 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.446.

  24. Tiffany ST: A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev. 1990, 97: 147-68. 10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.147.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Field M, Santarcangelo M, Sumnall H, Goudie A, Cole J: Delay discounting and the behavioural economics of cigarette purchases in smokers: The effects of nicotine deprivation. Psychopharmacol. 2006, 186: 255-63. 10.1007/s00213-006-0385-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Giordano LA, Bickel WK, Loewenstein G, Jacobs EA, Marsch L, Badger GJ: Mild opioid deprivation increases the degree that opioid-dependent outpatients discount delayed heroin and money. Psychopharmacol. 2002, 163: 174-182. 10.1007/s00213-002-1159-2.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Mitchell SH: Effects of short-term nicotine deprivation on decision-making: Delay, uncertainty and effort discounting. Nic Tob Res. 2004, 6: 819-828. 10.1080/14622200412331296002.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Epstein LH, Bulik CM, Perkins KA, Caggiula AR, Rodefer J: Behavioral economic analysis of smoking: Money and food as alternatives. Pharm Biochem Behav. 1991, 38: 715-721. 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90232-Q.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Griffiths RR, Rush CR, Puhala KA: Validation of the multiple choice procedure for investigating drug reinforcement in humans. Exp Clin Psychopharm. 1996, 4: 97-106. 10.1037/1064-1297.4.1.97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Perkins KA, Epstein LH, Grobe J, Fonte C: Tobacco abstinence, smoking cues, and the reinforcing value of smoking. Pharm Biochem Behav. 1994, 47: 107-112. 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90118-X.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Willner P, Hardman S, Eaton G: Subjective and behavioral evaluation of cigarette cravings. Psychopharm. 1995, 118: 171-177. 10.1007/BF02245836.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Perkins KA, Grobe J, Fonte C: Influence of acute smoking exposure on the subsequent reinforcing value of smoking. Exp Clin Psychopharm. 1997, 5: 277-85. 10.1037/1064-1297.5.3.277.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Sayette MA, Martin CS, Wertz JM, Shiffman S, Perrott MA: A multi-dimensional analysis of cue-elicited craving in heavy smokers and tobacco chippers. Addiction. 2001, 96: 1419-1432. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961014196.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Wise RA: The neurobiology of craving: Implications for the understanding and treatment of addiction. J Abnorm Psychol. 1987, 97: 118-132. 10.1037/0021-843X.97.2.118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Berridge KC, Robinson TE: What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience?. Brain Res Rev. 1998, 28: 309-369. 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Ciccocioppo R: The role of serotonin in craving: From basic research to human studies. Alcohol Alcohol. 1999, 34: 244-253.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. McGeary JE, Monti PM, Rohsenow DJ, Tidey J, Swift R, Miranda R: Genetic moderators of naltrexone's effects on alcohol cue reactivity. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2006, 30: 1288-96. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00156.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Hutchison KE, McGeary JE, Smolen A, Swift RM, Bryan A: The DRD4 VNTR polymorphism moderates craving after alcohol consumption. Health Psychol. 2002, 139-146. 10.1037/0278-6133.21.2.139.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Hutchison KE, Wooden A, Swift RM, Smolen A, McGeary JE, Adler L, Paris L: Olanzapine reduces craving for alcohol: A DRD4 VNTR polymorphism by pharmacotherapy interaction. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2003, 28: 1882-1888. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300264.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Hutchison KE, Ray L, Sandman E, Rutter MC, Peters A, Davidson D, Swift R: The effect of olanzapine on craving and alcohol consumption. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006, 31: 1310-7.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. Van Tol HH, Wu CM, Guan HC, Ohara K, Bunzow JR, Civelli O, Kennedy J, Seeman P, Niznik HB, Jovanovic V: Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature. 1992, 358: 149-152. 10.1038/358149a0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buckwaldt S, Patterson A, Jovanovic V, Van Tol HH: Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. J Neurochem. 1995, 65: 1157-1165.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Czermak C, Lehofer M, Liebmann PM, Traynor J: [35S]GTPgammaS binding at the human dopamine D4 receptor variants hD4.2, hD4.4 and hD4.7 following stimulation by dopamine, epinephrine and norepinephrine. Eur J Pharmacol. 2006, 381: 20-4. 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.11.063.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Van Craenenbroeck K, Clark SD, Cox MJ, Oak JN, Liu F, Van Tol HH: Folding efficiency is rate-limiting in dopamine D4 receptor biogenesis. J Biol Chem. 2005, 280: 19350-7. 10.1074/jbc.M414043200.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hutchison KE, Lachance H, Niaura R, Bryan A, Smolen A: The DRD4 VNTR polymorphism influences reactivity to smoking cues. J Abnorm Psychol. 2002, 111: 134-143. 10.1037/0021-843X.111.1.134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Shao C, Li Y, Jiang K, Zhang D, Xu Y, Lin L, Wang Q, Zhao M, Jin L: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism modulates cue-elicited heroin craving in Chinese. Psychopharmacol. 2006, 186: 185-90. 10.1007/s00213-006-0375-6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Sobik L, Hutchison K, Craighead L: Cue-elicited craving for food: A fresh approach to the study of binge eating. Appetite. 2004, 44: 253-261. 10.1016/j.appet.2004.12.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Carter BL, Tiffany ST: Meta-analysis of cue reactivity in addiction research. Addiction. 1999, 94: 327-340. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.9433273.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Rohsenow DJ, Monti PM, Abrams DB, Rubonis AV, Niaura RS, Sirota AD, Colby SM: Cue elicited urge to drink in alcoholics and salivation: Relationship to individual differences. Adv Behav Res Ther. 1992, 14: 195-210. 10.1016/0146-6402(92)90008-C.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Dawson DA: Alcohol consumption, alcohol dependence, and all-cause mortality. Alc Clin Exp Res. 2000, 24: 72-81. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2000.tb04556.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Meilman PW, Presley CA: Average weekly alcohol consumption: Drinking percentiles for American college students. J Am Coll Health. 1997, 45: 201-205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Babor TF, Higgins-Biddle JC, Saunders JB, Monteiro MG: Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test: Guidelines for Use in Primary Care. 2001, Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization

    Google Scholar 

  53. Collins LR, Parks GA, Marlatt GA: Social determinants of alcohol consumption: The effects of social interaction and model status on the self administration of alcohol. J Cons Clin Psychol. 1985, 53: 189-200. 10.1037/0022-006X.53.2.189.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  54. Kivlahan DR, Marlatt GA, Fromme K, Coppel DB, Williams E: Secondary prevention with college drinkers: Evaluation of an alcohol skills training program. J Cons Clin Psychol. 1990, 58: 805-810. 10.1037/0022-006X.58.6.805.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Sayette MA, Loewenstein G, Kirchner TR, Travis T: Effects of smoking urge on temporal cognition. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005, 19: 88-93. 10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.88.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Kozlowski LT, Mann RE, Wilkinson DA, Poulos CX: "Cravings" are ambiguous: Ask about urges or desires. Addict Behav. 1990, 14: 443-445. 10.1016/0306-4603(89)90031-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Kozlowski LT, Wilkinson DA: Use and misuse of the concept of craving by alcohol, tobacco, and drug researchers. Br J Addict. 1987, 82: 31-45. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1987.tb01430.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A: Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988, 54: 1063-70. 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Schmukle SC, Egloff B, Burns LR: The relationship between positive and negative affect in the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). J Res Pers. 2002, 36: 463-475. 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00007-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Bickel WK, Marsch LA: Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: Delay discounting processes. Addiction. 2001, 96: 73-86. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2001.961736.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Higgins ST, Bickel WK, Hughes JR: Influence of an alternative reinforcer on human cocaine self-administration. Life Sci. 1994, 55: 179-187. 10.1016/0024-3205(94)00878-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Zwaan RA, Stanfield RA, Madden CJ: How persistent is the effect of smoking urges on cognitive performance?. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2000, 8: 518-23. 10.1037/1064-1297.8.4.518.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Staiger PK, White JM: Cue reactivity in alcohol abusers: Stimulus specificity and extinction of the responses. Addict Behav. 1991, 16: 211-221. 10.1016/0306-4603(91)90014-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Lichter JB, Barr CL, Kennedy JL, Van Tol HH, Kidd KK, Livak KJ: A hypervariable segment in the human dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) gene. Hum Molec Genet. 1993, 2: 767-773. 10.1093/hmg/2.6.767.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Wertz JA, Sayette MA: A review of the effects of perceived drug use opportunity on self-reported urge. Exp Clin Psychopharm. 2001, 9: 3-13. 10.1037/1064-1297.9.1.3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Maude-Griffin P, Tiffany ST: Production of smoking urges through imagery: The impact of affect and smoking abstinence. Exp Clin Psychopharm. 1996, 4: 198-208. 10.1037/1064-1297.4.2.198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Payne TJ, Rychtarik RG, Rappaport NB, Smith PO: Reactivity to alcohol-relevant beverage and imaginal cues in alcoholics. Addict Behav. 1992, 17: 209-217. 10.1016/0306-4603(92)90026-R.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Baron RW, Kenny DA: The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986, 51: 1173-1182. 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Hutchison KE, Stallings M, McGeary J, Bryan A: Population stratification in the candidate gene study: Fatal threat or red herring?. Psychol Bull. 2004, 130: 66-79. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.1.66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Jacobs EA, Bickel WK: Modeling drug consumption in the clinic using simulation procedures: Demand for heroin and cigarettes in opioid-dependent outpatients. Exp Clin Psychopharm. 1999, 7: 412-426. 10.1037/1064-1297.7.4.412.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Murphy JG, MacKillop J: Relative reinforcing efficacy of alcohol among college student drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharm. 2006, 14: 219-227. 10.1037/1064-1297.14.2.219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Oak JN, Oldenhof J, Van Tol HHM: The dopamine D4 receptor: One decade of research. Eur J Pharm. 2000, 405: 303-327. 10.1016/S0014-2999(00)00562-8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Choi KH, Whisler DL, Graham DL, Self DW: Antisense-induced reduction in nucleus accumbens cyclic AMP response element binding protein attenuates cocaine reinforcement. Neurosci. 2006, 137: 373-383. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.049.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  74. Knapp CM, Lee K, Foye M, Ciraulo DA, Kornetsky C: Additive effects of intra-accumbens infusion of the cAMP-specific phosphodiesterae inhibitor, rolipam, and cocaine on brian stimulation reward. Life Sci. 2001, 69: 1673-1682. 10.1016/S0024-3205(01)01249-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Lynch WJ, Taylor JR: Persistent changes in motivation to self-administer cocaine following modulation of cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase A (PKA) activity in the nucleus accumbens. Eur J Neurosci. 2005, 22: 1214-1220. 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04305.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Svingos AL, Periasamy S, Pickel VM: Presynaptic dopamine D4 receptor localization in the rat nucleus accumbens shell. Synapse. 2000, 36: 222-232. 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2396(20000601)36:3<222::AID-SYN6>3.0.CO;2-H.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Erblich J, Lerman C, Self DW, Diaz GA, Bovbjerg DH: Effects of dopamine D2 receptor (DRD2) and transporter (SLC6A3) polymorphisms on smoking cue-induced cigarette craving among African-American smokers. Mol Psychiatry. 2005, 10: 407-14. 10.1038/sj.mp.4001588.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. van den Wildenberg E, Wiers RW, Dessers J, Janssen RG, Lambrichs EH, Smeets HJ, van Breukelen GJ: A functional polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor gene (OPRM1) influences cue-induced craving for alcohol in male heavy drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007, 31: 1-10. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00258.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Brody AL, Mandelkern MA, London ED, Childress AR, Lee GS, Bota RG, Ho ML, Saxena S, Baxter LR, Madsen D, Jarvik ME: Brain metabolic changes during cigarette craving. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002, 59: 1162-72. 10.1001/archpsyc.59.12.1162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. George MS, Anton RF, Bloomer C, Teneback C, Drobes DJ, Lorberbaum JP, Nahas Z, Vincent DJ: Activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior thalamus in alcoholic subjects on exposure to alcohol-specific cues. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001, 58: 345-52. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.4.345.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Myrick H, Anton RF, Li X, Henderson S, Drobes D, Voronin K, George MS: Differential brain activity in alcoholics and social drinkers to alcohol cues: relationship to craving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004, 29: 393-402. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300295.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Tapert SF, Brown GG, Baratta MV, Brown SA: fMRI BOLD response to alcohol stimuli in alcohol dependent young women. Addict Behav. 2004, 29: 33-50. 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Tapert SF, Cheung EH, Brown GG, Frank LR, Paulus MP, Schweinsburg AD, Meloy MJ, Brown SA: Neural response to alcohol stimuli in adolescents with alcohol use disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003, 60: 727-35. 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Grusser SM, Wrase J, Klein S, Hermann D, Smolka MN, Ruf M, Weber-Fahr W, Flor H, Mann K, Braus DF, Heinz A: Cue-induced activation of the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex is associated with subsequent relapse in abstinent alcoholics. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004, 175: 296-302. 10.1007/s00213-004-1828-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  85. Sanfey AG, Loewenstein G, McClure SM, Cohen JD: Neuroeconomics: cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006, 10: 108-16. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Drs. Peter M. Monti and Steven J. Lynn for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this article, and thank Chinatsu McGeary for assistance with genotyping. Portions of these data were previously presented at the 39th annual meeting of the Association for the Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, Washington, DC, and the 28th annual meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism, Santa Barbara, CA. This research was supported in part by grant T32 AA007459-20 from the National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to James MacKillop.

Additional information

Competing interests

The author(s) declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

JM conceived the study, developed the procedures, analyzed the data and was the principal author of the manuscript. DM contributed to the conception of the study, ran all participants, and contributed to data interpretation and manuscript preparation. JMc genotyped all participants, contributed to data interpretation, and participated in manuscript preparation. SAL contributed to the conception of the study, data interpretation, and manuscript preparation. All authors have read and approved this manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

MacKillop, J., Menges, D.P., McGeary, J.E. et al. Effects of craving and DRD4 VNTR genotype on the relative value of alcohol: an initial human laboratory study. Behav Brain Funct 3, 11 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-11

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-3-11

Keywords