Skip to main content

Individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease show differential patterns of ERP brain activation during odor identification

Abstract

Background

Studies suggest that older adults at risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease may show olfactory processing deficits before other signs of dementia appear.

Methods

We studied 60 healthy non-demented individuals, half of whom were positive for the genetic risk factor the Apolipoprotein E ɛ4 allele, in three different age groups. Event-related potentials to visual and olfactory identification tasks were recorded and analyzed for latency and amplitude differences, and plotted via topographical maps.

Results

Varying patterns of brain activation were observed over the post-stimulus epoch for ɛ4- versus ɛ4+ individuals on topographical maps. Individuals with the ɛ4 allele demonstrated different ERP peak latencies during identification of olfactory but not visual stimuli. High correct ApoE classification rates were obtained utilizing the olfactory ERP.

Conclusions

Olfactory ERPs demonstrate functional decline in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease at much earlier ages than previously observed, suggesting the potential for pre-clinical detection of AD at very early stages.

Alzheimer’s disease is a neurologic disorder accompanied by progressive memory loss, cognition loss and functional decline [1]. The cause or causes of AD are not yet known and definitive diagnosis can only be made via postmortem autopsy or, while living, a brain biopsy. The greatest risk factor for development of AD is advancing age. Genetic research has confirmed that the ɛ4 allele of the apolipoprotein E (ApoE) gene is the strongest genetic risk factor for AD [25]. Inheritance of a single ApoE ɛ4 variant increases a persons risk of developing AD by a factor of three in men and four in women, and having two copies of the ɛ4 allele increases risk up to 15-fold compared to persons without the ɛ4 variant [6, 7]. Presence of the ɛ4 allele increases the risk but does not guarantee future development of AD [8].

Studies have found olfactory dysfunction in AD including impairment in olfactory threshold sensitivity, odor identification, odor recognition memory, remote memory for odors, and odor fluency for review see [9]. Regions of the brain involved in the processing of olfactory information, such as the entorhinal cortex, prepiriform cortex, and the anterior olfactory nucleus show increased neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in AD, as well as cell loss, granulovacuolar degeneration and tangles in the olfactory bulb [4, 1015]. The neuropathological changes associated with AD have been shown to affect the primary regions of the brain involved in olfaction but have less effect on other primary sensory areas [16]. Greater hippocampal atrophy has been reported in non-demented ɛ4+ individuals compared to ɛ4- controls [17]. Studies of persons with the ɛ4 allele have also demonstrated olfactory deficits in odor identification [18], odor detection [19] and odor memory [20], as well as odor recognition memory [21]. Odor identification appears to be particularly sensitive to cognitive changes associated with dementia. Correct classification rates of 100% have been obtained between persons at risk for AD from controls utilizing an odor identification test [22]. ɛ4+ individuals demonstrate significantly poorer odor identification than ɛ4- nondemented older adults [18, 23]. Odor identification abilities declined more rapidly in ɛ4+ persons than ɛ4- persons over a four year time period while during the same time period there was no significant change in odor threshold, picture identification, or DRS scores [24]. Odor identification has been shown to be directly related to left hippocampal volume and to AD pathology in the brain [25, 26]. Given that areas of the brain that process olfactory information are some of the earliest affected in AD and those at risk for AD, olfactory changes may be some of the earliest signs of the disease in the preclinical phase.

Neuroimaging studies have suggested a functional recruitment hypothesis of age-related compensatory changes where those with AD and those at risk for AD utilize additional cognitive resources to bring memory-related performance to normal levels [2733]. Persons with a positive family history (FH) of AD and those with both FH and the ɛ4 allele had greater activation predominantly in the bilateral posterior cingulate/precuneus, bilateral temporoparietal junction, and bilateral prefrontal cortex [34]. ApoE+ individuals produced greater brain activation in the bilateral fusiform gyri, right superior parietal cortex, left pyramis/uvula, left middle frontal gyrus, and medial frontal gyrus [29]. Similarly, participants diagnosed with Mild Cognitive Impairment or AD demonstrate greater activation in the frontal areas of the brain [35]. These studies suggest the potential for detection of AD and early preclinical stages using measures of brain response.

Brain activity can be measured from the surface of the scalp via the electroencephalogram and more specifically the event-related potential (ERP), a measure that is exquisitely sensitive to the timing of the brain’s response. Olfactory event-related potentials (OERPs) recorded in relation to olfactory stimulation has demonstrated sensitivity to subtle changes in olfactory functioning associated with aging, disease, and ApoE status [3645]. OERPs require odor stimulation via specially built olfactometers which control the exact timing of stimulus onset while avoiding simultaneous stimulation of other sensory modalities such as stimulation of the trigeminal system [39, 4649]. These olfactometers also warm and humidify the air stream in order to prevent somatosensory cues. Reaction times to odors vary based on the stimulus and subject characteristics but range from 800-900 ms [50]. Neuronal recovery time of the olfactory system is much longer than other sensory systems [38, 51, 52]. Auditory and visual stimuli can be presented every 2–3 seconds in ERP research without significant adaptation [5355] while in the olfactory system inter-stimulus intervals of 30–45 seconds are required. This slower neuronal recovery is partially due to olfactory receptor cells that rapidly adapt and slowly recover [56] and partially due to habituation [52]. Given longer inter-stimulus intervals in olfactory stimulation, fewer trials are presented than in other systems in order to reduce potential subject fatigue and loss of vigilance. A narrower filter is also applied when processing the ERP data to compensate for the smaller number of trials.

The early components of the OERP, the N1, P2, and N2 are considered exogenous sensory components that have been associated with odor threshold and odor identification [38, 49, 57]. The P3 component in general represents endogenous processing of a stimulus, reflecting both stimulus classification speed and the ability to attend to and evaluate a stimulus [58, 59]. OERP P3 latency correlates with neuropsychological tests that measure memory and cognitive processing speed [60]. Several studies have demonstrated increased OERP peak latencies associated with aging [36, 38, 39, 41, 60]. Older males produced significantly smaller OERP peak amplitudes than older females when utilizing relatively short inter-stimulus intervals, suggesting greater olfactory impairments in males [38]. Studies of the OERP have further documented olfactory deficits in AD [61], specifically longer P2 and P3 latencies in AD patients compared to controls. These latency measures also correlate significantly with dementia status as measured by the Dementia Rating Scale (DRS). Importantly, studies utilizing the OERP with persons at risk for AD, due to the ApoE ɛ4 allele, have also demonstrated differences. ɛ4+ non-demented older adults produced significantly longer OERP latencies than age-matched ɛ4- individuals [62]. Additionally, high sensitivity and specificity was obtained in classifying ɛ4+ and ɛ4- individuals based on OERP latency alone. Utilizing a cross-modal odor recognition memory task differential brain activity was observed between ApoE groups, such that ɛ4- participants differed from ɛ4+ participants in activation of the frontal electrode sites, supporting the compensatory hypothesis [63].

This study examines OERPs in an odor identification task compared to a picture identification task in three separate age groups and in persons positive and negative for the ɛ4 allele. We hypothesized that ɛ4+ individuals would demonstrate differing topographical patterns of brain activation compared to ɛ4- individuals as measured by the ERP. As in previous studies we also hypothesized that ɛ4+ older adults would produce longer OERP peak latencies than ɛ4- participants and that this difference would be greater than differences measured with an odor threshold test and a traditional odor identification test (San Diego Odor Identification Test).

Methods and materials

Participants

Participants were 60 adults divided into three age groups, Young Adults (10 M, 10F, Mean age = 22.8 years), Middle Age Adults (10 M, 10F, Mean Age = 50.5), and Older Adults (10 M, 10F, Mean age = 70.7). Half of each group were positive for the ɛ4 allele. Table 1 presents demographic variables by age and ApoE groups. Participants were recruited from the general community, from San Diego State University, and from an ongoing subject pool at the Lifespan Human Senses Laboratory. The research was approved by IRBs at San Diego State University and the University of California, San Diego and subjects gave informed consent. All participants were screened for odor sensitivity via odor threshold test and odor identification test and any participants with threshold scores lower than 4, or odor identification scores less than 3, were excluded from the study [22, 64, 65]. Participants were screened for cognitive impairment using the Dementia Rating Scale, and any participants scoring less than 133 were excluded from the study [66]. Genetic DNA was obtained from each subject using buccal swab of cheek cells and analyzed for the APOE genotype at an offsite laboratory as described in [67]. Data from 40 of these participants have previously been published [68].

Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations for demographic, behavioral and performance data for each age and ApoE group

Procedure

San Diego Odor Identification Test (SDOIT)

The San Diego Odor Identification Test [22, 57, 69] consists of 8 common household odors (e.g. chocolate, coffee) presented in opaque jars. A set of 20 line drawings of the 8 odors and 14 distractors, presented in an array, were placed in front of each participant. Participants smelled the odors birhinically in random order and chose the odor from the array of drawings. Verbalizing the name of the odor or pointing to the picture of the odor were both acceptable ways of responding. Total number correct of the 6 most commonly identified odors was used for analysis.

ERP stimulus presentation

Olfactory stimulation was performed via computer controlled olfactometer incorporating designs of previous olfactometers [39, 4649]. Odors were presented utilizing a single stimulus paradigm for 200 msec and an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 30 sec. Participants employed Velopharyngeal closure to restrict breathing to the mouth and thereby maintain a constant odorant flow rate [46, 48, 70]. Fourteen separate odors (banana, rose, cinnamon, peanut butter, baby powder, mustard, chocolate, pine, lemon, orange, vanilla, coffee, leather, wintergreen) were presented twice each in pseudo-randomized order. Six of these odors were chosen in order to replicate the most identifiable odors from the San Diego Odor Identification Test [22, 57, 69]. All odorants were undiluted and two drops of each odorant were placed into the olfactometer before each subject session. After each odor presentation the participants were asked to identify the odor via button press from a list of four written options presented on the computer screen in front of them and their responses were recorded electronically.

In a separate experimental session on the same testing day, 28 visual line drawings of objects from the Boston Naming Test [71] were presented on a computer screen in front of the subject. Duration of each stimulus was 200 msec with 30 sec ISIs. As in the olfactory identification task the subjects were asked to identify each picture via button press from a list of four written options on the computer screen. Both olfactory and visual stimuli were presented via Compumedics STIM2 software. Order of experimental presentation, olfactory or visual, was randomized across subjects so that some subjects received the visual experiment first, and some the olfactory experiment first.

ERP recording

Olfactory and visual ERPs were obtained via Compumedics™ 64-electode AG/AG/CL sintered Quick-Cap and Quick-Cell system, amplified via Synamps 2 amplifiers, and recorded on computer hard disk via the Neuroscan software package. Electrode impedances were kept below 10 kΩ. At the time of recording the EEG data were digitized at 500 Hz through a 0.1 to 30 Hz bandpass filter. Offline, the data were further filtered through a 0.1 to 6 Hz bandpass filter. Artifactual eyeblink activity was recorded and corrected offline via the Neuroscan software utilizing the Occular Artifact Reduction method within the software. ERP trials that included other types of artifactual activity were excluded using both automated exclusion (e.g. excluding all trials with voltage ranges larger than 50 μV) and by visually inspecting each trial prior to averaging . Ongoing EEG activity was recorded throughout the experiment and then trials epoched offline to 500 msec pre-stimulus and 1500 msec post-stimulus. Baseline corrected trials were then averaged. Peak amplitudes were measured from the pre-stimulus baseline to maximum peak amplitude. Latency windows from previous OERP studies [38, 39] were used as guidelines to identify peak components. Peaks were picked blindly as to age, gender, and APOE status in order to avoid experimenter bias.

Results

Demographics and screening measures

Table 1 summarizes demographic and screening measures. Within each age group there were no significant differences in mean participant age between ApoE+ and ApoE- participants (p >.05). Dementia Rating Scale scores did not differ significantly between ApoE groups or age groups (p >.05) and all participants’ DRS scores were in the range of normal cognitive functioning. Analysis of odor threshold test performance revealed that all participants scored in the normal range of olfactory functioning. While all participants were normosmic, the older participant group exhibited poorer olfactory threshold scores than the middle and young age groups (F(2,57) = 8.93, p <.001, η2 = .24), with no interaction of ApoE status.

Odor and picture identification

Table 1 shows identification scores by age and ApoE groups. Analysis of the San Diego Odor Identification Test revealed no significant main effects or interaction effects involving ApoE status (p >.05). It did demonstrate a significant main effect of age (F(2,57) = 6.04, p <.01, η2 = .18) with young participants correctly identifying more odors than both the middle and older age groups (p <.05).

Analysis of correctly identified number of odors from the Odor Identification ERP task revealed a main effect of ApoE status collapsed across age groups (F(1,54) = 4.54, p <.05) η2 = .08), such that ɛ4- participants correctly identified more odors than ɛ4+ participants. The number of correctly identified odors did not differ significantly by ApoE status when each age group was analyzed separately, (suggesting that this effect is small when the screening measures are applied). Analysis of correctly identified pictures in the Picture Identification ERP task revealed a significant main effect of age group (F(2,54) = 3.60, p <.05, η2 = .12), such that older participants correctly identified more pictures than young participants (p <.05). The effect of ApoE status was not significant for Picture Identification.

Topographical displays of ERP activity

Figure 1 illustrates topographical distributions of OERP amplitudes in μV over the post-stimulus time interval from 700 ms through 1300 ms by age and ApoE groups. For each of 19 electrodes (FP1, FP2, F7, F3, F2, F4, F8, T7, C3, CZ, C4, T8, P7, P3, P2, P4, P8, O1, O2) amplitudes were averaged over the 100 ms time intervals (e.g. 700-800 ms) and input into graphing software in order to visually display brain activity across the scalp for each age and APOE group. Given no significant ApoE effects in the visual modality only olfactory ERP topographies are shown. In the young group the OERP topographical maps show greater brain activation in the ɛ4+ participants compared to ɛ4- participants, over the left hemisphere electrodes, and particularly over parietal electrodes, that decreases after 1100 ms. Middle age ɛ4+ and ɛ4- individuals demonstrated similar topographies, however ɛ4+ participants showed somewhat more activity over the right hemisphere electrodes compared to left, in the 900-1100 ms range, whereas ɛ4- participants showed more central electrode activity across the recording epoch. The greatest differences can be observed in the older groups where ɛ4- participants showed an increase in activation over left and central electrode sites between 900-1100 ms, and ɛ4+ participants showed relatively less overall activation during that time period, but increasing activation over right frontal electrodes between 1000-1200 ms. Overall the topographical maps clearly illustrate that brain activity related to olfactory processing differs not only by age, but more importantly by ApoE status, and the activity differentially changes over the post-stimulus time period.

Figure 1
figure 1

Topographical representation of OERP amplitudes in μV across 19 electrode sites by age and ApoE groups for 700-1300 ms post-stimulus time intervals.

Event Related Potentials (ERPs)

Repeated measures multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) were performed for each ERP component (N1, P2, N2, P3) and olfactory and visual modalities were analyzed separately for each peak amplitude and latency. Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were applied to all MANOVAs. Significant main effects and interactions were further analyzed with post hoc Newman Keuls Multiple Range Tests (alpha 0.05). Significant visual and olfactory ERP effects are summarized in Table 2.

Table 2 Summary of analyses performed and effect sizes for peak component amplitudes and latencies

Picture identification ERPs

Visual N1, P2, and N2 amplitudes demonstrated no significant main or interaction effects (p >.05). Visual P3 amplitude demonstrated a significant main effect for electrode site (F(2,108) = 4.28, p <.05, η2 = .07) such that the Cz and Pz electrode sites produced larger P3 amplitudes than the Fz site.

Figure 2 illustrates visual ERP peak latencies. Visual N1 and P2 latencies did not demonstrate significant main or interaction effects (p >.05). Visual N2 latency demonstrated a significant main effect of electrode site (F(2,108) = 3.50, p <.05, η2 = .06) with the Pz electrode site producing shorter N2 latencies than the Fz electrode site. Visual P3 latency also revealed a significant main effect of electrode site (F(2,108) = 8.78, p <.01, η2 = .14) with the Cz and Pz electrode sites recording significantly longer latencies than the Fz site (p <.01). Visual P3 latency also showed a significant main effect of age group (F(2,54) = 4.94, p <.05, η2 = .15) with older adults producing significantly longer visual P3 latencies than young adults (p <.01).

Figure 2
figure 2

Visual and olfactory average N1, P2, N2, and P3 latencies by age and ApoE groups. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean (SEM).

Odor identification ERPs

Olfactory N1, P2, and P3 amplitudes demonstrated no significant main or interaction effects (p >.05). Olfactory N2 amplitude demonstrated a significant main effect of age (F(2,54) = 3.62, p <.05, η2 = .12) with older age participants producing significantly more negative N2 amplitudes than middle age participants.

Figure 2 illustrates olfactory ERP peak latencies. Olfactory N1, P2, N2, and P3 latencies demonstrated significant interaction effects of Age x ApoE status (N1: F(2,54) = 4.73, p <.05, η2 = .15; P2: F(2,54) = 9.34, p <.001, η2 = .26; N2: F(2,54) = 8.18, p <.01, η2 = .23; P3: F(2,54) = 14.11, p <.001, η2 = .34). Post hoc analyses of the interaction effects revealed that in the young group those negative for the ɛ4 allele produced significantly longer N2 (η2 = .26), and P3 (η2 = .29), latencies. In the middle age group those positive for the ɛ4 allele produced significantly longer N1 (η2 = .31), and P2 (η2 = .31) latencies. In the older group those positive for the ɛ4 allele produced significantly longer latencies for all components N1 (η2 = .50), P2 (η2 = .52), N2(η2 = .35), and P3(η2 = .59). For N1 and P3 latencies, in both ApoE groups, older participants produced significantly longer latencies than middle age participants and middle age participants produced longer latencies than young participants. For P2 and N2 latencies in ɛ4+ participants, this same pattern was observed, however in ɛ4- participants young adults did not differ significantly from middle age participants, but both young and middle age participants produced shorter latencies than older participants.

Correlational analyses were performed between the ERP odor ID performance and average (Fz, Cz, Pz) peak latencies (N1, P2, N2, P3). When all ages and ApoE groups were combined together odor ID performance marginally correlated with N1 latency (r = −.28, p <.05). Correlational analyses were also performed for each age x ApoE group separately. The only significant correlation between odor ID performance and latency was for N1 latency in the older Apoe+ group (r = −.77, p <.01).

Logistic regression analyses of OERP variables

In order to better understand the predictive value of the ERP in differentiating ɛ4- and ɛ4+ participants, stepwise logistic regression analysis was performed on olfactory N1, P2, N2, P3 amplitude and latency averaged over Fz, Cz, and Pz. Visual ERP variables were not included in logistic regression analysis because no significant ApoE effects were present. A logistic regression is a type of regression analysis used to predict the outcome of a binary dependent variable (e.g. Apoe+ vs Apoe-) based on one or more predictor variables (e.g. ERP amplitude and latency). The logistic regression analysis outputs predictive classification results including overall correct classification (total percentage of correctly classified individuals), as well as sensitivity (e.g. APOE+ correctly classified as APOE+) and specificity (e.g. APOE- correctly classified as APOE-). In the olfactory modality analysis of all age groups combined revealed that olfactory N1 latency was the most significant single predictor in discriminating between ɛ4+ participants and ɛ4- participants (χ2 = 6.07, p <.05) resulting in an overall correct classification rate of 70.0% (Sensitivity = 66.7%, Specificity = 73.3%). When P3 latency was also added to N1 amplitude in the model (χ2 = 8.06, p <.01) overall correct classification rate increased to 76.7% (Sensitivity = 80%, Specificity = 73.3%). Logistic regressions were also performed for each age group separately in order to better understand the effects of ApoE status within each age group. In the young participant group N2 amplitude was the most significant predictor (χ2 = 7.79, p <.01) resulting in an overall classification rate of 65.0% (Sensitivity = 70.0%, Specificity = 60.0%). In the middle age group P2 latency was the most significant single predictor (χ2 = 6.86, p <.01) resulting in an overall classification rate of 80.0% (Sensitivity = 80.0%, Specificity = 80.0%). When P3 amplitude and P3 latency were also added to the equation with P2 latency (χ2 = 20.09, p <.001), the overall classification rate for middle age participants increased to 90.0% (Sensitivity = 90.0%, Specificity = 90.0%). In the older age group P3 latency was the most significant single predictor (χ2 = 16.37, p <.001) resulting in an overall classification rate of 90.0% (Sensitivity = 90.0%, Specificity = 90.0%). When N1 latency was also added to the equation with P3 latency (χ2 = 27.73, p <.001), overall classification rate for older participants increased to 100.0% (Sensitivity = 100.0%, Specificity = 100.0%).

Discussion

This study demonstrated (1) robust odor identification ERP differences based on ApoE status and interactions with age; (2) high correct ApoE classification rates utilizing the OERP that were different for each age group.

A few previous studies have demonstrated ERP impairments in persons with a positive family history of AD, in those diagnosed with early AD, and in individuals with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). MCI is commonly defined as subtle but measurable memory impairment without any other symptoms of dementia. Green et al. [72] demonstrated auditory ERP P3 latency increases in pre-clinical groups of persons with a family history of AD and in those with a family history plus ɛ4+ status. They did not indicate, however, how many of the participants demonstrated this latency difference, or how well this measure correctly classified participants into ɛ4+ or ɛ4- groups. Olichney et al. [73] studied participants diagnosed with MCI utilizing an N400/P600 semantic congruency task and then tracked those participants over time. Participants with abnormal N400 or P600 effects had an 87 to 88% likelihood of progression to dementia within 3 years. They suggest that these N400 abnormalities in MCI may reflect subtle dysfunction of semantic memory processes. Utilizing this method, classification of participants into diagnostic groups was high in sensitivity in MCI who converted to AD (81-94%) less so when applied to all participants (58 to 65%). Chapman et al. [74] used a visual number-letter memory task to study ERPs in participants diagnosed as being in the early stages of AD. Their results suggest that AD deficits may include problems with storage in short-term memory, and that difficulties may lie in the cognitive processing of stimuli that are relevant to the task in which the participant is engaged. Using this method the authors were able to correctly classify 92% of participants into AD or control groups. While these studies are extremely helpful in understanding cognitive changes associated with early stages of dementia, even at the MCI stage, they also raise the possibility of early diagnosis at even earlier, pre-MCI, pre-clinical, stages of AD. A measure that would aid diagnosis of AD many years before any manifestation of other signs or symptoms would be ideal, especially once an effective medication for halting or altering the dementing process is developed.

Illustrated on the ERP topographical maps, the current study demonstrated differing patterns of brain activity recorded over the scalp, depending on ApoE status. fMRI studies on activation have reported mixed results, with some showing increased activation in ɛ4 carriers [30, 75], and others demonstrating reduced activation in ɛ4 carriers [76]. The current study demonstrates that patterns of brain activation differ not only by ApoE status, but across age groups, and that the pattern of activation changes across time over the post-stimulus interval of cognitive processing. This suggests that processing of olfactory stimuli is differentially affected by presence or absence of the ɛ4 allele. Additionally these effects change over the lifespan, such that individuals in different decades of life, even young adults, show varying patterns of brain activation.

In the present study, it is important to note that in order to better understand the effects of the ApoE risk factor at points before memory problems are present, participants with dementia, early dementia, or mild cognitive impairment, as well as those with anosmia or severe hyposmia, were screened out of the study. Given this rigorous screening process, it is not surprising that no significant ApoE effects were found for the visual picture identification task. Very robust effects of ApoE status were demonstrated for the odor identification ERP task, as well as significant age by ApoE interaction effects. This occurred despite no difference in performance on the Dementia Ratings Scale, odor threshold testing, or the SDOIT by ApoE status. The present study strongly suggests that combining olfactory processing with cognitive processing [odor identification] may be sensitive enough to differentiate very early those with a high likelihood from those with a low likelihood of developing AD, even as early as middle age. This together suggests the ability to identify odors and the speed of odor identification is slowed in the presence of the ɛ4 allele. Picture identification remained intact in ApoE+ participants, both for number of pictures correctly identified and speed with which the pictures were cognitively processed.

Of further note, the present study demonstrated good ApoE classification rates of 80% in the middle age group, with ɛ4+ participants producing significantly longer N1 and P2 latencies than ɛ4- participants. It suggests that ApoE-related olfactory functional decline is taking place at much earlier ages than previously observed, further suggesting that early, pre-clinical, diagnosis of AD may be a real possibility. Consistent with previous OERP studies [62, 63], older ɛ4+ participants produced significantly longer OERP peak latencies. In this group 100% classification rates were obtained suggesting a clear delineation in performance between ɛ4+ and ɛ4- participants once they reach ages above 65 years old.

Further research needs to examine the utility of various measures and tasks in the study of pre-clinical AD, in order to capture the pre-dementing processes at the earliest possible stages, and improve diagnostic ability. Olfactory tasks involving cognitive processing, such as the OERP, appear to be very promising in this regard.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, Revised 4th ed. 2000, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press

    Google Scholar 

  2. Blacker D: The genetics of Alzheimer's disease: progress, possibilities, and pitfalls. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1997, 5 (4): 234-237. 10.3109/10673229709000306.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Combarros O, Alvarez-Arcaya A, Sanchez-Guerra M, Infante J, Berciano J: Candidate gene association studies in sporadic Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2002, 14 (1): 41-54. 10.1159/000058332.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Small GW, Roses AD, Haines JL, Pericakvance MA: Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science. 1993, 261 (5123): 921-923. 10.1126/science.8346443.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Teter B, Raber J, Nathan B, Crutcher KA: The presence of apoE4, not the absence of apoE3, contributes to AD pathology. J Alzheimers Dis. 2002, 4: 155-163.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Bertram L, Tanzi RE: The genetic epidemiology of neurodegenerative disease. J Clin Invest. 2005, 115 (6): 1449-1457. 10.1172/JCI24761.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, Myers RH, Pericakvance MA, Risch N, vanDuijn CM: Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease - A meta-analysis. JAMA. 1997, 278 (16): 1349-1356. 10.1001/jama.1997.03550160069041.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Burke W, Pinsky LE, Press NA: Categorizing genetic tests to identify their ethical, legal, and social implications. Am J Med Genet. 2001, 106 (3): 233-240. 10.1002/ajmg.10011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Murphy C: Loss of olfactory function in dementing disease. Physiol Behav. 1999, 66 (2): 177-182. 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00262-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Averback P: 2 new lesions in Alzheimers-disease. Lancet. 1983, 2 (8360): 1203-

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Braak H, Braak E: Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol Aging. 1997, 18: 351-357. 10.1016/S0197-4580(97)00056-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Christen-Zaech S, Kraftsik R, Pillevuit O, Kiraly M, Martins R, Khalili K, Miklossy J: Early olfactory involvement in Alzheimer's disease. Can J Neurol Sci. 2003, 30 (1): 20-25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Esiri MM, Wilcock GK: The Olfactory bulbs in Alzheimers-disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1984, 47 (1): 56-60. 10.1136/jnnp.47.1.56.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Ohm TG, Braak H: Olfactory-Bulb Changes in Alzheimers-Disease. Acta Neuropathol. 1987, 73 (4): 365-369. 10.1007/BF00688261.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Reyes PF, Golden GT, Fariello RG, Fagel L, Zalewska M: Olfactory pathways in Alzheimer's disease (AD): Neuropathological studies [abstract]. Society for Neuroscience. 1985, 11: 168-

    Google Scholar 

  16. Van Hoesen GW, Solodkin A: Cellular and systems neuroanatomical changes in Alzheimer's disease. In: Calcium hypothesis of aging and dementia. Disterhoft JF, Khachaturian ZS (eds). Proc NY Acad Sci (USA). 1994, 747: 12-35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. den Heijer T, Oudkerk M, Launer LJ, Van Duijn CM, Hofman A, Breteler MM: Hippocampal, amygdalar, and global brain atrophy in different apolipoprotein E genotypes. Neurology. 2002, 59: 746-748. 10.1212/WNL.59.5.746.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Murphy C, Bacon AW, Bondi MW, Salmon DP: Apolipoprotein E status is associated with odor identification deficits in nondemented older persons. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998, 855: 744-750. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb10654.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Bacon AW, Bondi MW, Salmon DP, Murphy C: Very early changes in olfactory functioning due to Alzheimer's disease and the role of apolipoprotein E in olfaction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998, 855: 723-731. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb10651.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Schiffman SS, Graham BG, Sattely-Miller EA, Zervakis J, Welsh-Bohmer K: Taste, smell and neuropsychological performance of individuals at familial risk for Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2002, 23 (3): 397-404. 10.1016/S0197-4580(01)00337-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Gilbert PE, Murphy C: Differences between recognition memory and remote memory for olfactory and visual stimuli in nondemented elderly individuals genetically at risk for Alzheimer's disease. Exp Gerontol. 2004, 39 (3): 433-441. 10.1016/j.exger.2004.01.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Morgan CD, Nordin S, Murphy C: Odor Identification as an Early Marker for Alzheimers-Disease - Impact of Lexical Functioning and Detection Sensitivity. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1995, 17 (5): 793-803. 10.1080/01688639508405168.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Olofsson JK, Nordin S, Wiens S, Hedner M, Nilsson LG, Larsson M: Odor identification impairment in carriers of ApoE-varepsilon4 is independent of clinical dementia. Neurobiol Aging. 2010, 31 (4): 567-577. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.05.019.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Calhoun-Haney R, Murphy C: Apolipoprotein epsilon 4 is associated with more rapid decline in odor identification than in odor threshold or Dementia Rating Scale scores. Brain Cogn. 2005, 58 (2): 178-182. 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.10.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Murphy C, Jernigan TL, Fennema-Notestine C: Left hippocampal volume loss in Alzheimer's disease is reflected in performance on odor identification: a structural MRI study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2003, 9 (3): 459-471.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Wilson RS, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Boyle PA, Buchman AS, Bennett DA: Olfactory Impairment in Presymptomatic Alzheimer's Disease. Ann N Y Aca Sci. 2009, 1170 (1): 730-735(736). 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04013.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Backman L, Andersson JL, Lyberg L, Winblad B, Nordberg A, Almkvist O: Brain regions associated with episodic retrieval in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1999, 52 (9): 1861-1870. 10.1212/WNL.52.9.1861.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Becker JT, Mintun MA, Aleva K, Wiseman MB, Nichols T, DeKosky ST: Compensatory reallocation of brain resources supporting verbal episodic memory in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1996, 46 (3): 692-700. 10.1212/WNL.46.3.692.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Bondi MW, Houston WS, Eyler LT, Brown GG: fMRI evidence of compensatory mechanisms in older adults at genetic risk for Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2005, 64 (3): 501-508. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000150885.00929.7E.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Bookheimer SY, Strojwas MH, Cohen MS, Saunders AM, Pericak-Vance MA, Mazziotta JC, Small GW: Patterns of brain activation in people at risk for Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med. 2000, 343 (7): 450-456. 10.1056/NEJM200008173430701.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Grady CL, McIntosh AR, Beig S, Keightley ML, Burian H, Black SE: Evidence from functional neuroimaging of a compensatory prefrontal network in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurosci. 2003, 23 (3): 986-993.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Saykin AJ, Flashman LA, Frutiger SA, Johnson SC, Mamourian AC, Moritz CH, O’Jile JR, Riordan HJ, Santulli RB, Smith CA, Weaver JB: Neuroanatomic substrates of semantic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: patterns of functional MRI activation. J Int Neuropscyhol Soc. 1999, 5 (5): 377-392. 10.1017/S135561779955501X.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Woodard JL, Grafton ST, Votaw JR, Green RC, Dobraski ME, Hoffman JM: Compensatory recruitment of neural resources during overt rehearsal of word lists in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 1998, 12 (4): 491-504.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Seidenberg M, Guidotti L, Nielson KA, Woodard JL, Durgerian S, Antuono P, Zhang Q, Rao SM: Semantic memory activation in individuals at risk for developing Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2009, 73 (8): 612-620. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b389ad.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, van Meel CS, Scheltens P: Alterations in brain activation during cholinergic enhancement with rivastigmine in Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002, 73 (6): 665-671. 10.1136/jnnp.73.6.665.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Covington JW, Geisler MW, Polich J, Murphy C: Normal aging and odor intensity effects on the olfactory event-related potential. Int J Psychophysiol. 1999, 32 (3): 205-214. 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00012-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Evans WJ, Cui L, Starr A: Olfactory event-related potentials in normal human subjects: effects of age and gender. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1995, 95 (4): 293-301. 10.1016/0013-4694(95)00055-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Morgan CD, Covington JW, Geisler MW, Polich J, Murphy C: Olfactory event-related potentials: older males demonstrate the greatest deficits. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1997, 104 (4): 351-358. 10.1016/S0168-5597(97)00020-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Morgan CD, Geisler MW, Covington JW, Polich J, Murphy C: Olfactory P3 in young and older adults. Psychophysiology. 1999, 36 (3): 281-287. 10.1017/S0048577299980265.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Morgan CD, Murphy C: Differential effects of active attention and age on event-related potentials to visual and olfactory stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 2010, 78 (2): 190-199. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.07.008.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Murphy C, Morgan CD, Geisler MW, Wetter S, Covington JW, Madowitz MD, Nordin S, Polich JM: Olfactory event-related potentials and aging: normative data. Int J Psychophysiol. 2000, 36 (2): 133-145. 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00107-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Nordin S, Quinonez C, Morgan CD, Geisler MW, Polich J, Murphy C: Olfactory event-related potentials in young and elderly adults: evaluation of tracking task versus eyes open/closed recording. Chem Senses. 1999, 24 (4): 459-464. 10.1093/chemse/24.4.459.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Thesen T, Wetter S, Murphy C: Olfactory event-related potential detects age-related changes in olfactory processing with velopharyngeal closure and normal breathing. Psychophysiology. 2000, 37: S97-

    Google Scholar 

  44. Wetter S, Murphy C: Individuals with Down's syndrome demonstrate abnormal olfactory event-related potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. 1999, 110 (9): 1563-1569. 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00086-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Wetter S, Peavy G, Jacobson M, Hamilton J, Salmon D, Murphy C: Olfactory and auditory event-related potentials in Huntington's disease. Neuropsychology. 2005, 19 (4): 428-436.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Kobal G: Elektrophysiologische Untersuchungen des menschlichen Geruchsinns. 1981, Stuttgart: Thieme

    Google Scholar 

  47. Lorig TS, Elmes DG, Zald DH, Pardo JV: A computer-controlled olfactometer for fMRI and electrophysiological studies of olfaction. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 1999, 31 (2): 370-375. 10.3758/BF03207734.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Lorig TS: The application of electroencephalographic techniques to the study of human olfaction: a review and tutorial. Int J Psychophysiol. 2000, 36 (2): 91-104. 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00104-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Murphy C, Nordin S, de Wijk RA, Cain WS, Polich J: Olfactory-evoked potentials: assessment of young and elderly, and comparison to psychophysical threshold. Chem Senses. 1994, 19 (1): 47-56. 10.1093/chemse/19.1.47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Overbosch P, de Wijk R, de Jonge TJ, Koster EP: Temporal integration and reaction times in human smell. Physiol Behav. 1989, 45 (3): 615-626. 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90082-6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Ekman GB, Berglund U, Berglund B, Lindwall T: Perceived intensity of odor: a function of time of adaptation. Scan J Psychol. 1967, 8: 177-186. 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1967.tb01392.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Wilson DA, Linster C: Neurobiology of simple memory. J Neurophysiol. 2008, 100: 2-7. 10.1152/jn.90479.2008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Polich J: P300, probability, and inter-stimulus interval. Psychophysiology. 1990, 27: 396-403. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1990.tb02333.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Polich J: Probability and inter-stimulus interval effects on the P300 from auditory stimuli. Int J Psychophysiol. 1990, 10: 163-170. 10.1016/0167-8760(90)90030-H.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Polich J: P300 in clinical applications: meaning, method, and measurement. Electroencephalography: Basic Principles, Clinical Applications, and Related Fields. Edited by: Niedermeyer E, Lopes da Silva F. 1993, Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins, 1005-1018. 3

    Google Scholar 

  56. Moore PA: A model of the role of adaptation and disadaptation in olfactory receptor neurons: implications for the coding of temporal and intensity patterns in odor signals. Chem Senses. 1994, 19 (1): 17-86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Murphy C, Morgan CD: Olfactory Function and Event-Related Potentials in Alzheimers Disease. Alzheimer’s Disease: Advances in Etiology, Pathogenesis and Therapeutics. Edited by: Iqbal K, Sisodia SS, Winblad B. 2001, Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd, 237-251.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Donchin E, Heffley E, Hillyard SA, Loveless N, Maltzman I, Ohman A, Rosler F, Ruchkin D, Siddle D: Cognition and event-related potentials. II. The orienting reflex and P300. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1984, 425: 39-57. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23522.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Polich J, Hoffman LD: P300 and handedness: on the possible contribution of corpus callosal size to ERPs. Psychophysiology. 1993, 35 (5): 497-507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Geisler MW, Morgan CD, Covington JW, Murphy C: Neuropsychological performance and cognitive olfactory event-related brain potentials in young and elderly adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1999, 21 (1): 108-126. 10.1076/jcen.21.1.108.935.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Morgan CD, Murphy C: Olfactory event-related potentials in Alzheimer's disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2002, 8 (6): 753-763. 10.1017/S1355617702860039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Wetter S, Murphy C: Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 positive individuals demonstrate delayed olfactory event-related potentials. Neurobiol Aging. 2001, 22 (3): 439-447. 10.1016/S0197-4580(01)00215-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Murphy C, Solomon ES, Haase L, Wang M, Morgan CD: Olfaction in aging and Alzheimer's disease: event-related potentials to a cross-modal odor-recognition memory task discriminate ApoE epsilon4+ and ApoE epsilon 4- individuals. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009, 1170: 647-657. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04486.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Cain WS, Gent J, Catalanotto FA, Goodspeed RB: Clinical evaluation of olfaction. Am J Otolaryngol. 1983, 4 (4): 252-256. 10.1016/S0196-0709(83)80068-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Murphy C, Gilmore MM, Seery CS, Salmon DP, Lasker BR: Olfactory thresholds are associated with degree of dementia in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 1990, 11 (4): 465-469. 10.1016/0197-4580(90)90014-Q.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Mattis S: Mental status examination for organic mental syndrome in the elderly patient. Geriatric psychiatry: A handbook for psychiatrists and primary care physicians. Edited by: Bellak L, Katasu TB. 1976, New York: Grune and Statton, 77-121.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Sundermann EE, Gilbert PE, Murphy C: Apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 genotype and gender: Effects on memory. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007, 15 (10): 869-878. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318065415f.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Zamora R, Bartholow J, Green E, Morgan CD, Murphy C: Adiposity measures predict olfactory processing speed in older adult carriers of the apolipoprotein E4 allele. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011, 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.001.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Murphy C, Schubert CR, Cruickshanks KJ, Klein BE, Klein R, Nondahl DM: Prevalence of olfactory impairment in older adults. JAMA. 2002, 288 (18): 2307-2312. 10.1001/jama.288.18.2307.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Thesen T, Murphy C: Age-related changes in olfactory processing detected with olfactory event-related brain potentials using velopharyngeal closure and natural breathing. Int J Psychophysiol. 2001, 40 (2): 119-127. 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00157-4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Kaplan E, Goodglass H, Weintrab S: The Boston Naming Test. 1983, Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger

    Google Scholar 

  72. Green J, Levey AI: Event-related potential changes in groups at increased risk for Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 1999, 56 (11): 1398-1403. 10.1001/archneur.56.11.1398.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Olichney JM, Taylor JR, Gatherwright J, Salmon DP, Bressler AJ, Kutas M, Iragui-Madoz VJ: Patients with MCI and N400 or P600 abnormalities are at very high risk for conversion to dementia. Neurology. 2008, 70 (19): 1763-1770.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Chapman RM, Nowlis GH, McCrary JW, Chapman JA, Sandoval TC, Guillily MD, Gardner MN, Reilly LA: Brain event-related potentials: diagnosing early-stage Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. 2007, 28 (2): 194-201. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.12.008.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Han SD, Houston WS, Jak AJ, Eyler LT, Nagel BJ, Fleisher AS, Brown GG, Cory-Bloom J, Salmon D, Thal LJ, Bondi MW: Verbal paired-associate learning by APOE genotype in non-demented older adults: fMRI evidence of a right hemispheric compensatory response. Neurobiol Aging. 2007, 28 (2): 238-247. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.12.013.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Lind J, Persson J, Ingvar M, Larsson A, Cruts M, Van Broeckhoven C, Adolfsson R, Backman L, Nilsson LG, Petersson KM, Nyberg L: Reduced functional brain activity response in cognitively intact apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 carriers. Brain. 2006, 129: 1240-1248. 10.1093/brain/awl054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Supported by NIH Grant DC002064 to Claire Murphy. The authors would like to thank the late Dr. Leon Thal and the UCSD ADRC (P50AG005131-28) for genotyping, Dr. John Polich, Krystin Corby, Joel Kowalewski, Jessica Bartholow, and Roberto Zamora for research assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claire Murphy.

Additional information

Competing interest

The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Authors’ contributions

CDM and CM designed the study and advised the research assistants through data collection and analysis, and interpreted the data. CDM constructed the olfactometer. CDM and CM wrote and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Morgan, C.D., Murphy, C. Individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease show differential patterns of ERP brain activation during odor identification. Behav Brain Funct 8, 37 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-37

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-37

Keywords