Skip to main content

Preliminary evidence about the effects of meditation on interoceptive sensitivity and social cognition

Abstract

Background

Interoception refers to the conscious perception of body signals. Mindfulness is a meditation practice that encourages individuals to focus on their internal experiences such as bodily sensations, thoughts, and emotions. In this study, we selected a behavioral measure of interoceptive sensitivity (heartbeat detection task, HBD) to compare the effect of meditation practice on interoceptive sensitivity among long term practitioners (LTP), short term meditators (STM, subjects that completed a Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) program) and controls (non-meditators). All participants were examined with a battery of different tasks including mood state, executive function and social cognition tests (emotion recognition, empathy and theory of mind).

Findings

Compared to controls, both meditators’ groups showed lower levels of anxiety and depression, but no improvement in executive function or social cognition performance was observed (except for lower scores compared to controls only in the personal distress dimension of empathy). More importantly, meditators’ performance did not differ from that of nonmeditators regarding cardiac interoceptive sensitivity.

Conclusion

Results suggest no influence of meditation practice in cardiac interoception and in most related social cognition measures. These negative results could be partially due to the fact that awareness of heartbeat sensations is not emphasized during mindfulness/vipassana meditation and may not be the best index of the awareness supported by the practice of meditation.

Background

Interoception involves the conscious perception of feelings from inside the body [13]. Interoception has been proposed to modulate social cognition processes such as motivational behavior [2], empathy [4], and theory of mind (ToM), which have been suggested to be supported by emotional and body feedback information [4].

Meditation is a form of mental training [5] encouraging individuals to focus on their internal experiences, such as bodily sensations, thoughts, and emotions [6]. One component of meditation involves the development of interoceptive attention to visceral sensations [7]. Additionally, meditation practice promotes the development of prosocial behavior [8].

Previous findings reported no difference in interoception accuracy between meditators and nonmeditators [5, 9]. In these studies, a heartbeat discrimination paradigm was selected: participants had to discriminate whether their heartbeats synchronized with either auditory or visual cues [10]. Consequently, subjects had to attend at the same time to their cardiac sensation and to external stimuli which have been shown to affect interoceptive performance [11]. We selected a different heartbeat detection paradigm [12] to avoid the possible interference of external stimuli. Moreover, given the relationship between interoception and social cognition [2, 4, 13, 14], we included tasks of emotion recognition, empathy and ToM to test the association among bodily perception, social cognition and meditation practice. Moreover, considering the interaction between executive functions (EF) and social cognition domains (emotional processing [15], ToM [16] and empathy [17]), EF abilities were also evaluated.

Our aim was to compare the effect of meditation practice on interoceptive sensitivity and related measures among long term practitioners (LTP), subjects that completed a Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) program (short term meditators, STM) and controls (nonmeditators). We predicted that long term practitioners would show enhanced interoceptive sensitivity, reflected in a better performance in heartbeat detection and related domains of social cognition.

Methods

Subjects

Ten nonmeditators, 9 short-term meditators and 10 long-term practitioners participated. The LTP group’s mean was 4.35 (SD = 2.17) years of continued practice and the STM completed an 8-week Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) program (see criteria in the Additional file 1: Table S1). Controls had never attended a yoga or meditation course. Groups were age, gender and education matched. We controlled body mass index because it influences the interoceptive performance [18]. Participants had no history of drug abuse, neurological or psychiatric conditions. Participants provided an informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and the study was approved by the institutional ethics committee.

Neuropsychological and clinical evaluation

Participants completed the Beck’s Depression Inventory (BDI) and the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) to evaluate mood and affective state, respectively. EF were assessed with the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) [19] indexing 8 EF (see Additional file 1: Table S1) and the Stroop test.

Social cognition tasks

A description of social cognition tasks (empathy, theory of mind and emotion recognition) is provided in Table 1 (see also Additional file 1: Table S1 for a detailed explanation of the materials and methods).

Table 1 Interoception and social cognition domain assessed and tasks employed

Interoception

Heartbeat detection task (HBD)

The HBD is a motor tracking test that assesses interoception sensitivity [12]. Participants had to tap a key on a keyboard along with their heartbeat in different conditions (see Table 1 and the Additional file 1: Table S1 data for a more detailed explanation).

Data analysis

Demographic, neuropsychological, and experimental data were compared among groups using ANOVA and Tukey’s HSD post-hoc tests. For categorical variables (e.g., gender), Kruskal-Wallis tests were applied. Mixed repeated measured ANOVA was performed for HBD, with a within-subject factor (the four conditions) and a between-subject factor (group).

Results

Demographic and neuropsychological results

No differences were found in gender [H = 4.90, p = 0.86], age [F (2, 25) = 0.95, p = 0.39, ηp2 0.07], formal education [F (2, 25) = 2.13, p = 0.13, ηp2 = 0.14] or body mass index [F (2, 21) = 1.47, p = 0.25, ηp2 =0.12] among groups.

Groups showed similar EF performance measured by the IFS [F (2, 25) = 1.50, p = 0.24, ηp2 =0.10]. There were no differences in the three condition of the Stroop task, word [F (2, 23) = 0.20, p = 0.81, ηp2 =0.01], color [F (2, 23) = 1.40, p = 0.26, ηp2 =0.10] and incongruent color-word [F (2, 23) = 0.35, p = 0.70, ηp2 =0.03]. No interference effect was found [F (2, 23) = 1.88, p = 0.17, ηp2 =0.14] (See Table 2).

Table 2 Demographic, neuropsychological and clinical results

Clinical evaluation

We observed a significant difference for BDI score among groups [F (2, 25) = 4.12, p < 0.05,, ηp2 = 0.24]. Post-hoc comparisons (Tukey HSD test, MS = 34.97; df = 25.00) revealed higher scores of depressive symptoms in controls compared to STM (p < 0.05). We did not observe between group differences for STAI-State subscale [F (2, 25) = 1.87, p = 0.17, ηp2 = 0.13]. However, significant differences for STAI-Trait subscale [F (2, 25) = 3.74, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.23] were observed; post hoc comparisons (Tukey test, HSD, MS = 69.98; df = 25.00) showed controls had significantly higher anxiety scores (p < 0.05) than LTM.

Social cognition measures

Emotion recognition: No differences were observed regarding total accuracy [F (2, 25) = 2.49, p = 0.10, ηp2 = 0.16]. However, per category analysis showed significant differences in disgust recognition among groups [F (2, 25) = 4.1, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.24]. A post-hoc comparison (Tukey HSD test, MS = 0.01; df = 25.00) revealed lower accuracy performance in LTM group (p < 0.05) than controls (see Figure 1a). Groups did not differ regarding RTs of average emotions recognition [F (2, 25) = 1.84, p = 0.17, ηp2 = 0.12]. Conversely, significant differences among groups were observed for disgust recognition [F (2, 25) = 3.97, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.24]. Post-hoc comparisons showed significantly slower RT for controls than STM group (p < 0.5). No other differences were observed (see Figure 1b).

Figure 1
figure 1

Social cognition. Emorphing. Percent of accuracy (a) and reaction times in seconds (b) are depicted for every basic emotion and for average scores. Interpersonal reactivity index (IRI). Raw scores of each subscales are presented (c). Reading the mind in the eyes (ToM) Total scores (d). *indicates significant differences.

Empathy: Group differences were found in Personal distress subscale [F (2, 25) = 7.88, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.38]. A post-hoc comparison (Tuckey HSD, MS = 13.53; df = 25.00) showed that both LTM and STM groups scored lower than controls (p < 0.01, for both). No other difference was observed (see Figure 1c).

Theory of mind (ToM): No group differences were observed [F (2, 25) = 1.10, p = 0.34, ηp2 = 0.08] (see Figure 1d).

Interoception

No group effects [F (2,25) = 0.57, p = 0.57, ηp2 = 0.04] or condition × group interaction [F (6, 75) = 0.59, p = 0.72, ηp2 = 0.04] were observed. Thus, there were no significant differences in the ability to track their heartbeats (interoceptive conditions) or an external cued heartbeat (motor and feedback conditions), in any of the four conditions (See Figure 2). Only an expected [12] and irrelevant effect of condition was observed (see Additional file 2: Table S2).

Figure 2
figure 2

Heartbeat detection task (HBD). The Accuracy Index can vary between 0 and 1, with higher scores indicating better accuracy. No differences were found among groups in any condition. Vertical bars indicate standard deviation.

Discussion

This is the first study assessing the influence of meditation practice both in cardiac interoception and in social cognition using a range of tasks. We selected a HBD task that avoids the possible interference of external stimuli [11] previously reported [5, 9].

No differences in EF or demographic variables were observed. Related to mood and affective scales, controls showed higher STM (depression) and LTM (anxiety-trait) scores. These results might reflect the possible influence of skills acquired during meditation practice (without considering its length), such as stress coping and emotional regulation abilities, which could help to deal with anxiety and depression situations. These skills might modulate mood perception as more euthymic and positive [20].

Regarding interoception, we replicated negative results previously reported [5, 9]. Body awareness includes one internal (viscera and blood composition) and one external stream (taste, smell, pressure sensations and pain [21]). Consequently, cardiac sensations might be considered as a basic modality of visceral perception that relies mostly on internal drive (the heart being an internal organ), which is why it would be more difficult to gain conscious inspection. Respiration is unique among interoceptive signals as it involves external pressure information from the nose and chest, and it is susceptible of voluntary control and straightforward conscious perception. During meditation, attention is commonly directed towards breathing [5], where more consistent results have been shown [2, 7]. These findings suggest that cardiac perception might not be the most suitable index to reflect meditation influence on interoception.

Few group differences were observed in social cognition domains. The lower accuracy in disgust recognition found in LTM compared to controls might be related to their lower cardiac interoceptive sensitivity (given the common insular involvement for interoception and disgust recognition [22]). However, this is speculative because interoceptive differences were not significant among groups.

Both meditators’ groups showed significantly lower empathy scores compared to controls only in the personal distress subscale, an index of emotional contagion by others’ distress [23]. This is unsurprising since one of the aims of meditation is the regulation of responsiveness to stressors [24]. Finally, no difference in ToM was observed. Overall, despite the few differences reported, groups have similar social cognition performance suggesting that meditation practice in this study may not impact on these abilities.

Our study suffers from important limitations. First, the sample size should be increased to allow more informative analysis (i.e. correlations, multiple regressions) about the association among meditation, interoception and social cognition. However, it is worth highlighting that we reported preliminary data about interoception sensitivity measure with a novel method, and that previous research has employed similar sample size [9]. Second, further studies should cover a multidimensional interoceptive assessment (not only cardiac but also breathing, cardiac, visceral, etc.) and including both awareness and sensibility dimensions. Finally, groups’ homogeneity should be guaranteed by measuring variables that might bias visceral perception such as physical state, volume stroke, blood pressure and contractibility (Additional file 2: Table S2).

Conclusion

In conclusion, no influence of meditation practice in cardiac interoception and related social cognition measures was observed. Based on the existence of diverse interoceptive signals, a more extensive assessment of each visceral source (other than cardiac one) may be necessary to disentangle the influence of meditation on interoceptive sensitivity.

Authors’ information

Margherita Melloni, Lucas Sedeño as the first author.

Abbreviations

HBD:

Heartbeat detection task

LTP:

Long term practitioners

STM:

Short term meditators

MBSR:

Mindfulness-based stress reduction

ToM:

Theory of mind

EF:

Executive functions

BDI:

Beck’s depression inventory

STAI:

State trait anxiety inventory

IFS:

INECO frontal screening

IRI:

Interpersonal reactivity index.

References

  1. Cameron OG: Interoception: the inside story–a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosom Med. 2001, 63: 697-710.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Craig A: How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002, 3: 655-666.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Barrett LF, Quigley KS, Bliss-Moreau E, Aronson KR: Interoceptive sensitivity and self-reports of emotional experience. Journal Personality Social Psychology. 2004, 87: 684-697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Lamm C, Singer T: The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Struct Funct. 2010, 214: 579-591. 10.1007/s00429-010-0251-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Khalsa SS, Rudrauf D, Damasio AR, Davidson RJ, Lutz A, Tranel D: Interoceptive awareness in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology. 2008, 45: 671-677. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00666.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Hölzel B, Lazar S, Gard T, Schuman-Olivier Z, Vago D, Ott U: How Does Mindfulness Meditation Work? Proposing Mechanisms of Action From a Conceptual and Neural Perspective. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011, 6: 537-559. 10.1177/1745691611419671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Farb NA, Segal ZV, Anderson AK: Mindfulness meditation training alters cortical representations of interoceptive attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosci. 2013, 8: 15-26. 10.1093/scan/nss066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Vago DR, Silbersweig DA: Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012, 6: 296-

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Nielsen L, Kaszniak AW: Awareness of subtle emotional feelings: a comparison of long-term meditators and nonmeditators. Emotion. 2006, 6: 392-405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Whitehead WE, Drescher V, Heiman P: Relation of heart rate control to heartbeat perception. Biofeedback Self Regul. 1977, 2: 371-392. 10.1007/BF00998623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Pennebaker JW: The psycholgy of phisical symptoms. 1982, New York: Springer

    Book  Google Scholar 

  12. Couto B, Salles A, Sedeño L, Peradejordi M, Barttfeld P, Canales-Johnson A, Dos Santos YV, Huepe D, Bekinschtein T, Sigman M: The man who feels two hearts: the different pathways of interoception. Social Cognitive and Affective Neurosci. 2013, published on line

    Google Scholar 

  13. Brass M, Haggard P: The hidden side of intentional action: the role of the anterior insular cortex. Brain Struct Funct. 2010, 214: 603-610. 10.1007/s00429-010-0269-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Dunn BD, Galton HC, Morgan R, Evans D, Oliver C, Meyer M, Cusack R, Lawrence AD, Dalgleish T: Listening to your heart. How interoception shapes emotion experience and intuitive decision making. Psychol Sci. 2010, 21: 1835-1844. 10.1177/0956797610389191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Pessoa L: On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008, 9: 148-158. 10.1038/nrn2317.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Ahmed FS, Stephen Miller L: Executive function mechanisms of theory of mind. J Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2011, 41: 667-678. 10.1007/s10803-010-1087-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Singer T, Lamm C: The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009, 1156: 81-96. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04418.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Rouse CH, Jones GE, Jones KR: The effect of body composition and gender on cardiac awareness. Psychophysiology. 1988, 25: 400-407. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1988.tb01876.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Lopez P, Manes F: INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): a brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. J The International Neuropsychological Society: JINS. 2009, 15: 777-786. 10.1017/S1355617709990415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Britton WB, Shahar B, Szepsenwol O, Jacobs WJ: Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy improves emotional reactivity to social stress: results from a randomized controlled trial. Behav Ther. 2012, 43: 365-380. 10.1016/j.beth.2011.08.006.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Cameron OG: Visceral Sensory Neuroscience: Interoception. 2002, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  22. Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G: Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron. 2003, 40: 655-664. 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Davis M: Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Personality and Social Psychology. 1983, 44: 113-126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Kabat-Zinn J, Nhat Hanh T: Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness. 1990, New York: Bantam Dell

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the “Asociación Mindfulness Argentina” for providing the experimental subjects and the place for examination made on this work. Specifically, we are grateful for Mrs Clara Badino and Mr Julio Laurindo predisposition to participate and advice on meditator’s sample selection. We also thank all participants of this study. This research was partially supported by CONICET, INECO Foundation, CONICYT/FONDECYT Regular (1130920), FONCyT- PICT 2012–0412, FONCyT- PICT 2012–1309, and James McDonnell Foundation Grants. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of those grants.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Agustin Ibanez.

Additional information

Competing interests

All the authors declare that they have no competing interests with respect to this study or its publication.

Authors’ contributions

MM and LS collected the data, statistically analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. BC was involved in the study conception and design, writing the protocol and contributed to the drafting of the manuscript. MR contributed in collecting the data and revising the final version of the manuscript. CG contributed to writing the final version of the manuscript. ACJ and MS contributed to revising the final version of the manuscript. FM contributed to revising the final version of the manuscript. AI is the head of our laboratory, was involved in the study conception and design and contributed to writing the final version of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12993_2013_478_MOESM1_ESM.docx

Additional file 1: Methods. In this Additional file 1 we provide a supplementary and detailed description of the materials and methods used in the study (DOCX 30 KB)

12993_2013_478_MOESM2_ESM.docx

Additional file 2: HBD additional results. In this Additional file 2 we provide a supplementary description of others interoceptive results. (DOCX 42 KB)

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Authors’ original file for figure 2

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Melloni, M., Sedeño, L., Couto, B. et al. Preliminary evidence about the effects of meditation on interoceptive sensitivity and social cognition. Behav Brain Funct 9, 47 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-9-47

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-9-47

Keywords