- Short Paper
- Open access
- Published:
Traumatic injury in female Drosophila melanogaster affects the development and induces behavioral abnormalities in the offspring
Behavioral and Brain Functions volume 15, Article number: 11 (2019)
Abstract
Traumatic injury (TI) during pregnancy increases the risk for developing neurological disorders in the infants. These disorders are a major concern for the well-being of children born after TI during pregnancy. TI during pregnancy may result in preterm labor and delivery, abruptio placentae, and/or fetomaternal hemorrhage. Drosophila melanogaster (fruit fly) is a widely used model to study brain and behavioral disorders in humans. In this study, we analyzed the effects of TI to female fruit flies on the development timing of larvae, social interaction and the behavior of offspring flies. TI to the female flies was found to affect the development of larvae and the behavior of offspring flies. There was a significant increase in the length of larvae delivered by traumatically injured maternal flies as compared to larvae from control maternal flies (without TI). The pupae formation from larvae, and the metamorphosis of pupae to the first generation of flies were faster in the TI group than the control group. Negative geotaxis and distance of the fly to its nearest neighbor are parameters of behavioral assessment in fruit flies. Negative geotaxis significantly decreased in the first generation of both male (p = 0.0021) and female (p = 0.0426) flies. The distance between the first generation of flies to its nearest neighbor was shorter in both male and female offspring flies in the TI group as compared to control group flies. These results indicate that TI to the female flies affected the development of larvae and resulted in early delivery, impaired social interaction and behavioral alterations in the offspring.
Background
The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimates the prevalence of developmental disorders at 1 in every 6 children [1]. Neurodevelopmental disorders such as intellectual disability, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and fragile X syndrome have core abnormal behavioral components that are fundamental to their diagnosis. Abnormal social interactions and impairments in verbal and non-verbal communication as well as repetitive and restricted behavior or interests are core components in autism diagnosis. In addition, other features are often associated with this triad, such as difficulties with decision-making [2].
Traumatic injury (TI) during pregnancy is a risk factor for neurological disorders in children. The injuries during pregnancy occur by motor vehicle accidents (49%), falls (25%), assaults (18%), gun shots (4%), and burns (4%) [3]. Up to 6–7% of pregnancies are affected by some degree of TI [4], which is usually accidental but may include intentional violence. TI during pregnancy may lead to preterm labor and delivery, abruptio placentae, and/or fetomaternal hemorrhage [5,6,7,8,9]. TI in pregnant women increases the risk for several mental and physical disabilities, which include developmental delay, vision and hearing loss, speech impairment, learning and communication difficulties, movement abnormalities, and mental health issues in the children [10,11,12,13].
Drosophila melanogaster is a widely used model for studies of brain disorders such as Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease, fragile X syndrome, and Angelman syndrome [14]. It is a comparatively simple organism, with its genome consisting of four chromosomes encoding approximately 14,000 genes [15, 16]. On the other hand, there are 20,000 protein and RNA- coding genes in the human genome [17]. Among 59 of the human neurological genes examined, 38 have orthologs in the Drosophila genome [18]. A single Drosophila gene may serve the same function as multiple related genes of mammals, thus decreasing the redundancy seen in other vertebrate models. Although flies and humans are distinctly different from each other, many molecular processes are conserved between them. The advantage of studying neurobehavioral disorders in Drosophila is the presence of genes that are similar to human genes for normal cognitive functions [19, 20]. The Drosophila model exhibits complex behaviors relevant to humans, including courtship [21, 22], circadian rhythms [23], learning and memory [24], aggression [25], grooming [26], and open field exploration [27]. It is also an attractive model because of its quicker generation time, large number of progeny for better selection, and easy maintenance.
There are differences between pregnant fruit flies and pregnant women. Fruit flies deliver their embryos, after which the development of embryo to fruit fly takes place outside, whereas embryo-to-infant development takes place inside the uterus in women. Therefore, the fruit fly is considered a good model to study the transition between stages, i.e., embryo; 1st, 2nd, and 3rd instar larva; and pupa to fly. We have previously reported that environmental risk factors cause neurobehavioral changes in Drosophila. In fruit flies, we observed that exposure to bisphenol A (BPA) affects behavior [28], and that methylmercury exposure inhibits alcohol dehydrogenase [29] and affects sexual functions [30].
In this study, we have examined the effects of TI to female flies on the development of larvae and the behavior of the first generation of flies. Climbing or negative geotaxis is an innate behavior of the fruit fly. There has been considerable interest in using this simple behavior to gain insight into the alterations in brain function [31]. Therefore, we compared negative geotaxis in TI and control offspring flies to study whether TI to female flies induces behavior abnormalities in the offspring. We also studied the effects of TI on social interaction among the offspring flies by measuring the distance between a fly and its nearest neighbor in a social space assay, as described previously [28, 32].
Materials and methods
Drosophila melanogaster
Wild-type Oregon-R Drosophila melanogaster stocks were maintained at 250 C on a standard cornmeal diet (Jazz-mix Drosophila food, Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) under 12Â h:12Â h light and dark cycle.
Traumatic injury to female flies
As shown in Fig. 1, 10-ml polypropylene tubes (Genesee Scientific; catalogue number 21-392) (Fig. 1A, #1) were cut below the cap (Fig. 1A, #2), and a cotton plug (Fig. 1A, #3) was placed. The tube was marked at 2 ml and 8 ml (Fig. 1A, #4) Twenty female flies (6–8 days old) were separated from male flies just before they were transferred to these tubes. The flies were brought down to the 2-ml mark on the tubes by tapping the tube and pushing down the cotton plug (Fig. 1A, #5). We induced TI to female flies following the method of Katenberger [33]. As described in Fig. 1B, C, the top end of the tube was slid in the spring to the 8-ml mark, and then the spring was released to hit the platform jack to induce TI to the flies.
The female flies were transferred to food vials at 7Â pm immediately after TI. At the same time, 20 control flies (without TI) of the same age were also transferred to food vials. The next day at 7 am, the flies were removed, and the development of larvae was studied.
In this report, larvae, pupae and offspring flies that emerged from maternal flies after TI are identified as TI-larvae, TI-pupae and TI flies. Similarly, larvae, pupae and flies obtained from control female flies are identified as C-larvae, C-pupae and C-flies.
Handling of larvae
When larvae were 4Â days old (3rd instar larvae), 20% sucrose solution was added to the food vials, and the floating larvae were removed after 5Â min. The larvae were placed on glass slides and kept in a refrigerator. Photographs of the larvae along with a scale were taken.
Measurement of length of larvae
The changes in larval length in Drosophila may indicate the changes in development. We measured the length of larvae using ImageJ NIH software (http://imagej.nih.gov/ij/). In brief, a file was opened in ImageJ. A straight line was selected, and a line (1-cm- long) on the scale was drawn, followed by clicking ‘analyze’ and ‘measure’. Next, ‘set scale’ was clicked; followed by clicking ‘change distance’ to 1 cm (do not change distance in pixels, and ‘aspect ratio’), and then ‘OK’ was clicked. Then ‘analyze’ followed by ‘measure’ was clicked. This technique produces a 1-cm-length measuring tool. Afterwards, a line (straight or free-hand line) was drawn on the larval length, and ‘measure’ was clicked each time, yielding the length of larvae in cm.
Time taken for origination of flies from pupae
The control and TI flies were placed in the food vials to lay eggs for 12Â h from 7Â pm to 7 am the next day. The flies were removed at 7 am, and pupae-to-fly conversion, i.e., eclosion of flies was counted each morning.
Negative geotaxis
In one set of experiment, 20 flies of the first generation (6–8 days old) were transferred to a Drosophila vial (narrow), and another vial was placed over it. These two vials were then joined with tape. To count the number of flies crossing 3/4th of the distance in the two vials, a line was drawn at the ¾th distance (14.5 cm) on the two vials. The tubes were tapped five times, and movement of the flies was video-recorded. The number of flies crossing the 14.5-cm mark was counted. One more set of experiments with different group of flies was conducted in a similar manner.
Social interaction
Social interaction was determined in male and female flies by using the social space assay, as we described previously [28]. The plastic chamber used for this assay had a triangle of 16 cm base and 17 cm height, which was covered from both sides by square plastic plates (18 cm × 18 cm). In brief, the first generation of flies (6–8 days old) from the control (n = 20) and TI groups (n = 20) were separated according to their gender, and transferred to the chamber. After the flies spent 40 min exploring and settling in the chamber, a digital image of the chamber with the flies was taken with a camera. Image J was then used to process the image into an 8-bit binary image. The binary image was then imported to the Lispix program (NIH image analysis software—http://www.nist.gov/lispix/) to calculate the distance of the fly to its nearest neighboring fly.
Data analysis
Data were analyzed for larval size by Student t test, and for negative geotaxis and social interaction (distance of the fly to its nearest neighbor fly) by one-way ANOVA using GraphPad Prism 10.
Results
Effect of traumatic injury on survival rate of female flies
It was observed that 20–30% of traumatically injured female flies died. Therefore, the number of maternal flies after TI was always less than the number of the control maternal flies. However, we used the same number of control and TI offspring flies in each assay.
Traumatic injury affects larval length
The larvae (4 days old) were collected and their length was measured as described in Methods section. TI to female flies resulted in a significant increase (p = 0.0365) in larval length (0.2478 ± 0.026 cm, mean ± SD, N = 20) as compared to that in control flies (0.2147 ± 0.31 cm, mean ± SD, N = 20) (Fig. 2). This data suggests that the development of the larvae gets accelerated in the TI group.
Traumatic injury in female flies causes early metamorphosis from pupa to fly
Metamorphosis of flies, i.e., pupa to fly conversion was studied in TI and control flies. In the TI female flies group, pupae formation was earlier by a day and the metamorphosis started on the 12th day after TI to female flies (30% of the first generation of flies were observed) and 100% of flies were seen by the 13th day. On the other hand, no first generation of flies were seen on the 12th day in the control female flies group. Metamorphosis in control flies was observed on the 13th and 14th days. Our results showed that TI to the female flies causes a shift in the timing of metamorphosis (Fig. 3). We also observed that total number of offspring flies in TI group were slightly less as compared to control group.
Effect of traumatic injury on negative geotaxis in first generation of flies
The movement of flies in an upward direction (climbing) is called negative geotaxis. The negative geotaxis assay is important for studying behavioral changes in fruit flies. TI in female flies affected the negative geotaxis in the first generation of both male and female flies (Fig. 4). A significant reduction in negative geotaxis was observed in both TI male (p = 0.0021) and TI female flies (p = 0.0426) compared to their respective control groups. Twenty male flies and 20 female flies in each control and TI group were studied. In male flies, 6.14 ± 2.4 TI flies (mean ± SD) crossed the ¾th mark as compared to 11.4 ± 2.7 (mean ± SD) flies in the control group. In female flies, 1.57 ± 0.78 TI flies (mean ± SD) crossed the ¾th mark as compared to 3.28 ± 1.1 (mean ± SD) flies in the control group. It was also observed that negative geotaxis was significantly greater in male flies than in female flies in both the TI (p = 0.0152) and the control group (p = 0.0007).
Abnormal social Interactions in first generation of male and female flies after traumatic injury to female flies
The distance between an individual fly and its closest neighbor has been used as a measure of social interaction within the group, as previously described [30, 34]. TI in female flies caused a significant effect on the distance maintained between the male and female flies (first generation) in a social group (Fig. 5). The first generation of male and female flies that originated from traumatically injured female flies was significantly closer in distance than in control flies (male flies, p = 0.0232; female flies, p = 0.049). Two sets of independent experiments with different flies were conducted in each group.
Discussion
It has been suggested that trauma complicates approximately 1 in 12 pregnancies [34], and it is the leading non-obstetric cause of maternal death [35]. Trauma also has fetal implications and has been reported to increase the incidence of spontaneous abortion, preterm premature rupture of membranes, preterm birth, uterine rupture, cesarean delivery, placental abruption and stillbirth [36,37,38,39,40]. The rate of fetal death from maternal trauma is 2.3 per 100,000 live births [39], with placental abruption as a major contributing factor [41]. It is reported that 1 in 3 pregnant women admitted for trauma to the hospital delivers during her hospitalization [42]. While pregnancy per se does not appear to increase morbidity or mortality due to trauma, the presence of a gravid uterus alters the pattern of injury [41].
Drosophila melanogaster has been used as an animal model for traumatic brain injury [33]. In this study, we used this model to study how TI in pregnant female flies affects the offspring. We had anticipated that TI to female flies would lead to a decrease in larval length. However, to our surprise, we observed an increase in TI-larval length. TI-larvae also had faster pupae formation and metamorphosis (pupa to fly conversion) as compared to control larvae. Because pupae formation was faster in TI-larvae, we also observed a shift in metamorphosis, i.e., early metamorphosis by at least 1Â day in TI-pupae than in the C-pupae. The reason for this rapid growth may be that TI in maternal flies led to faster delivery of embryos or that inflammation was increased as compared to control flies.
We also studied whether the behavior of offspring flies born after TI to female flies would be affected. Our results indicate that TI to female flies resulted in decreased negative geotaxis in both male and female flies, suggesting that TI in pregnant flies may also affect the behavior of the offspring. Not only was the negative geotaxis decreased but the distance between the nearest neighbors in flies was also decreased in the first generation of both male and female flies after TI to female flies as compared to control group without TI, suggesting that TI in flies leads to impaired social interaction in the offspring. We are not yet certain whether negative geotaxis has any direct link to social interaction, i.e., altered nearest neighbor distance of the flies. Since both these parameters are on behavioral changes in fruit fly, there is a possibility that alterations in these assays (negative geotaxis and social space assay) caused by TI to female flies may have some association.
One of the core features of autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders such as fragile X and Angelman’s syndromes is the inability of the individual to interact socially with other individuals [43,44,45], which is a diagnostic criterion in autism. It is also a common feature in studies using mouse models for autism [46]. Social behavior is the ability of conspecifics to interact, leading to changes in the subsequent behaviors of the individual [47,48,49]. In a social setting, the individual maintains a personal space or distance from another individual (personal space boundary), but also a spatial proximity to another individual for effective communication [50]. This social space or the space between two individuals of the same species is seen in most animals such as birds, fish, or locusts. When placed in a social setting, flies tend to arrange themselves uniformly rather than in aggregates or randomly [51, 52], and this social interaction in the group leads to learning of higher behaviors from their conspecifics [47, 52, 53]. Studies have shown that social isolation in Drosophila reduces the fiber number in the mushroom bodies, the functional equivalent of the hippocampus [54]; increases aggression [55,56,57]; and shortens the life span [57]. When placed in settings where they can freely interact with other flies, Drosophila flies usually maintain a distance of about two-body lengths (1–5 mm) among themselves, similar to other animals [32, 52]. This allows for the flies to orient themselves to interact with each other. Thus, interaction between flies follows a non-random pattern [58]. In our study, inter-fly distance was used as a measure for social interaction [59,60,61,62]. We have previously reported a decrease in the inter-fly distance when the flies exposed to BPA were placed in a social setting [28]. This decrease may be due to aberrant social interaction, in which the flies do not maintain the ideal balance of attraction/repulsion and interact inappropriately with each other. It is quite possible that children born after TI may also exhibit lack of social interaction features as observed in autism and fragile X syndrome.
Conclusion
In female fruit flies, TI affected the larval length and metamorphosis (pupa to fly conversion), thus affecting the development of offspring. In addition, the behavior of flies born after TI to maternal female flies was also affected as assessed by negative geotaxis and social interaction (distance between the fly and its nearest neighbor) assays. These results suggest that TI in female flies causes developmental abnormalities in offspring, which results in abnormal behavioral functions of adult flies. Therefore, TI during pregnancy in human may play a major role in affecting the development and behavior of children.
Data analysis
Data were analyzed for larval size by Student t-test, and for negative geotaxis and social interaction (distance of the fly to its nearest neighbor fly) by one-way ANOVA using GraphPad Prism 10. All the comparisons were tested and only significant comparisons are labelled.
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Abbreviations
- ASD:
-
autism spectrum disorder
- BPA:
-
Bisphenol A
- CDC:
-
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
- TI:
-
traumatic injury
References
Boyle CA, Boulet S, Schieve LA, Cohen RA, Blumberg SJ, Yeargin-Allsopp M, et al. Trends in the prevalence of developmental disabilities in US children 1997–2008. Pediatrics. 2011;127:1034–42.
Luke L, Clare IC, Ring H, Redley M, Watson P. Decision-making difficulties experienced by adults with autism spectrum conditions. Autism. 2012;16:612–21.
El-Kady D, Gilbert WM, Anderson J, et al. Trauma during pregnancy: an analysis of maternal and fetal outcomes in a large population. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2004;190:1661–8.
Peckham CH, King RW. A study of intercurrent conditions observed during pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1963;87:609–24.
Agran PF, Dunkle DE, Winn DG, et al. Fetal death in motor vehicle accidents. Ann Emerg Med. 1987;16:1355–8.
Drost TF, Rosemurgy AS, Sherman HF, et al. Major trauma in pregnant women: maternal/fetal outcome. J Trauma Injury Infect Crit Care. 1990;30:574–8.
Esposito TJ, Gens DR, Smith LG, et al. Evaluation of blunt abdominal trauma occurring during pregnancy. J Trauma Injury Infect Crit Care. 1989;29:1628–32.
Goodwin TM, Breen MT. Pregnancy outcome and fetomaternal hemorrhage after noncatastrophic trauma. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1990;162:665–71.
Lavin JP Jr, Polsky SS. Abdominal trauma during pregnancy. Clin Perinatol. 1983;10:423–38.
Shaw OEF, Yager JY. Preventing childhood and lifelong disability: maternal dietary supplementation for perinatal brain injury. Pharmacol Res. 2019;139:228–42.
Vexler ZS, Ferriero DM. Molecular and biochemical mechanisms of perinatal brain injury. Semin Neonatol. 2001;6(2):99–108.
Novak I, Hines M, Goldsmith S, Barclay R. Clinical prognostic messages from a systematic review on cerebral palsy. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5):e1285–312.
Shevell MI, Dagenais L, Hall N. The relationship of cerebral palsy subtype and functional motor impairment: a population-based study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51(11):872–7.
Mackay TF, Anholt RR. Of flies and man: Drosophila as a model for human complex traits. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2006;7:339–67.
Adams MD, Celniker SE, Holt RA, Evans CA, Gocayne JD, Amanatides PG, et al. The genome sequence of Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 2000;287:2185–95.
Celniker SE, Rubin GM. The Drosophila melanogaster genome. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2003;4:89–117.
Moraes F, Góes A. A decade of human genome project conclusion: scientific diffusion about our genome knowledge. Biochem Mol Biol Educ. 2016;44:215–23.
Rubin GM, Yandell MD, Wortman JR, Gabor Miklos GL, Nelson CR, Hariharan IK, et al. Comparative genomics of the eukaryotes. Science. 2000;287:2204–15.
Inlow JK, Restifo LL. Molecular and comparative genetics of mental retardation. Genetics. 2004;166:835–81.
Restifo LL. Mental retardation genes in Drosophila: new approaches to under-standing and treating developmental brain disorders. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2005;11:286–94.
Coen P, Clemens J, Weinstein AJ, Pacheco DA, Deng Y, Murthy M. Dynamic sensory cues shape song structure in Drosophila. Nature. 2014;507:233–7.
Dickson BJ. Wired for sex: the neurobiology of Drosophila mating decisions. Science. 2008;322:904–9.
Konopka RJ, Benzer S. Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1971;68:2112–6.
Tully T, Preat T, Boynton SC, Del Vecchio M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell. 1994;79:35–47.
Chen S, Lee AY, Bowens NM, Huber R, Kravitz EA. Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:5664–8.
Tauber JM, Vanlandingham PA, Zhang B. Elevated levels of the vesicular monoamine transporter and a novel repetitive behavior in the Drosophila model of fragile X syndrome. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e27100.
Burns JG, Svetec N, Rowe L, Mery F, Dolan MJ, Boyce WT, et al. Gene-environment interplay in Drosophila melanogaster: chronic food deprivation in early life affects adult exploratory and fitness traits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(Suppl 2):17239–44.
Kaur K, Simon AF, Chauhan V, Chauhan A. Effect of bisphenol A on Drosophila melanogaster behavior—a new model for the studies on neurodevelopmental disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2015;284:77–84.
Chauhan V, Chauhan A. Effects of methylmercury and alcohol exposure in Drosophila melanogaster: potential risks in neurodevelopmental disorders. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2016;51:36–41.
Chauhan V, Srikumar S, Aamer S, Pandareesh MD, Chauhan A. Methylmercury exposure induces sexual dysfunction in male and female Drosophila melanogaster. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(10):E1108.
Willenbrink AM, Gronauer MK, Toebben LF, Kick DR, Wells M, Zhang B. The Hillary Climber trumps manual testing: an automatic system for studying Drosophila climbing. J Neurogenet. 2016;30:205–11.
Simon AF, Chou MT, Salazar ED, Nicholson T, Saini N, Metchev S, et al. A simple assay to study social behavior in Drosophila: measurement of social space within a group. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11:243–52.
Katzenberger RJ, Loewen CA, Wassarman DR, Petersen AJ, Ganetzky B, Wassarman DA. A Drosophila model of closed head traumatic brain injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(44):E4152–9.
Hill CC, Pickinpaugh J. Trauma and surgical emergencies in the obstetric patient. Surg Clin North Am. 2008;88:421–40.
Fildes J, Reed L, Jones N, Martin M, Barrett J. Trauma: the leading cause of maternal death. J Trauma. 1992;32:643–5.
Pak LL, Reece EA, Chan L. Is adverse pregnancy outcome predictable after blunt abdominal trauma? Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1998;179:1140–4.
Schiff MA, Holt VL. Pregnancy outcomes following hospitalization for motor vehicle crashes in Washington state from 1989 to 2001. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161:503–10.
Weiss HB, Songer TJ, Fabio A. Fetal deaths related to maternal injury. JAMA. 2001;286:1863–8.
Pearlman MD, Tintinallli JE, Lorenz RP. A prospective controlled study of outcome after trauma during pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1990;162:1502–7.
Schiff MA, Holt VL, Daling JR. Maternal and infant outcomes after injury during pregnancy in Washington state from 1989 to 1997. J Trauma. 2002;53:939–45.
Shah KH, Simons RK, Holbrook T, Fortlage D, Winchell RJ, Hoyt DB. Trauma in pregnancy: maternal and fetal outcomes. J Trauma. 1998;45:83–6.
Kuo C, Jamieson DJ, McPheeters ML, Meikle SF, Posner SF. Injury hospitalizations of pregnant women in the United States, 2002. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2007;196(161):e1–6.
Wing L, Gould J. Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnor-malities in children: epidemiology and classification. J Autism Dev Disord. 1979;9:11–29.
Kaufmann WE, Cortell R, Kau AS, Bukelis I, Tierney E, Gray RM, et al. Autism spectrum disorder in fragile X syndrome: communication, social interaction, and specific behaviors. Am J Med Genet A. 2004;129A:225–34.
Wadge H, Brewer R, Bird G, Toni I, Stolk A. Communicative misalignment in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Cortex. 2019;115:15–26.
Jamain S, Radyushkin K, Hammerschmidt K, Granon S, Boretius S, Varo-queaux F, et al. Reduced social interaction and ultrasonic communication in a mouse model of monogenic heritable autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:1710–5.
Sokolowski MB. Social interactions in simple model systems. Neuron. 2010;65:780–94.
Novelli D, Drury J, Reicher S. Come together: two studies concerning the impact of group relations on personal space. Br J Soc Psychol. 2010;49:223–36.
Kohn NR, Reaume CJ, Moreno C, Burns JG, Sokolowski MB, Mery F. Social environment influences performance in a cognitive task in natural variants of the foraging gene. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e81272.
Sommer R. Studies in personal space. Sociometry. 1959;22:247–60.
Sexton OJ, Stalker HD. Spacing patterns of female Drosophila paramelanica. Anim Behav. 1961;9:77–81.
Billeter JC, Jagadeesh S, Stepek N, Azanchi R, Levine JD. Drosophila melanogaster females change mating behaviour and offspring production based on social context. Proc Biol Sci. 2012;279:2417–25.
Krupp JJ, Kent C, Billeter JC, Azanchi R, So AK, Schonfeld JA, et al. Social experience modifies pheromone expression and mating behavior in male Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol. 2008;18:1373–83.
Technau GM. Fiber number in the mushroom bodies of adult Drosophila melanogaster depends on age, sex and experience. J Neurogenet. 2007;21:183–96.
Hoffmann AA. The influence of age and experience with conspecifics on territorial behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. J Insect Behav. 1990;3:1–12.
Zhou C, Rao Y, Rao Y. A subset of octopaminergic neurons are important for Drosophila aggression. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:1059–67.
Ruan H, Wu CF. Social interaction-mediated lifespan extension of Drosophila Cu/Zn superoxide dismutase mutants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:7506–10.
Schneider J, Dickinson MH, Levine JD. Social structures depend on innate determinants and chemosensory processing in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(Suppl 2):17174–9.
Burg ED, Langan ST, Nash HA. Drosophila social clustering is disrupted by anesthetics and in narrow abdomen ion channel mutants. Genes Brain Behav. 2013;12:338–47.
Hahn N, Geurten B, Gurvich A, Piepenbrock D, Kastner A, Zanini D, et al. Monogenic heritable autism gene neuroligin impacts Drosophila social behavior. Behav Brain Res. 2013;252:450–7.
Bolduc FV, Valente D, Nguyen AT, Mitra PP, Tully T. An assay for social interaction in Drosophila Fragile X mutants. Fly (Austin). 2010;4:216–25.
Schneider J, Atallah J, Levine JD. One, two, and many—a perspective on what groups of Drosophila melanogaster can tell us about social dynamics. Adv Genet. 2012;77:59–78.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
This work was supported in part by funds from the New York State Office for People with Developmental Disabilities and the Autism Research Institute.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
VC planned the study, performed the experiments, analyzed data and wrote the manuscript. AC helped in planning the study, data analysis and manuscript preparation. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not applicable. The work is on Drosophila melanogaster, which does not require approval and consent.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Chauhan, V., Chauhan, A. Traumatic injury in female Drosophila melanogaster affects the development and induces behavioral abnormalities in the offspring. Behav Brain Funct 15, 11 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-019-0163-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-019-0163-1