Skip to main content

Traumatic injury in female Drosophila melanogaster affects the development and induces behavioral abnormalities in the offspring

Abstract

Traumatic injury (TI) during pregnancy increases the risk for developing neurological disorders in the infants. These disorders are a major concern for the well-being of children born after TI during pregnancy. TI during pregnancy may result in preterm labor and delivery, abruptio placentae, and/or fetomaternal hemorrhage. Drosophila melanogaster (fruit fly) is a widely used model to study brain and behavioral disorders in humans. In this study, we analyzed the effects of TI to female fruit flies on the development timing of larvae, social interaction and the behavior of offspring flies. TI to the female flies was found to affect the development of larvae and the behavior of offspring flies. There was a significant increase in the length of larvae delivered by traumatically injured maternal flies as compared to larvae from control maternal flies (without TI). The pupae formation from larvae, and the metamorphosis of pupae to the first generation of flies were faster in the TI group than the control group. Negative geotaxis and distance of the fly to its nearest neighbor are parameters of behavioral assessment in fruit flies. Negative geotaxis significantly decreased in the first generation of both male (p = 0.0021) and female (p = 0.0426) flies. The distance between the first generation of flies to its nearest neighbor was shorter in both male and female offspring flies in the TI group as compared to control group flies. These results indicate that TI to the female flies affected the development of larvae and resulted in early delivery, impaired social interaction and behavioral alterations in the offspring.

Background

The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimates the prevalence of developmental disorders at 1 in every 6 children [1]. Neurodevelopmental disorders such as intellectual disability, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism spectrum disorder (ASD), and fragile X syndrome have core abnormal behavioral components that are fundamental to their diagnosis. Abnormal social interactions and impairments in verbal and non-verbal communication as well as repetitive and restricted behavior or interests are core components in autism diagnosis. In addition, other features are often associated with this triad, such as difficulties with decision-making [2].

Traumatic injury (TI) during pregnancy is a risk factor for neurological disorders in children. The injuries during pregnancy occur by motor vehicle accidents (49%), falls (25%), assaults (18%), gun shots (4%), and burns (4%) [3]. Up to 6–7% of pregnancies are affected by some degree of TI [4], which is usually accidental but may include intentional violence. TI during pregnancy may lead to preterm labor and delivery, abruptio placentae, and/or fetomaternal hemorrhage [5,6,7,8,9]. TI in pregnant women increases the risk for several mental and physical disabilities, which include developmental delay, vision and hearing loss, speech impairment, learning and communication difficulties, movement abnormalities, and mental health issues in the children [10,11,12,13].

Drosophila melanogaster is a widely used model for studies of brain disorders such as Parkinson’s disease, Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease, fragile X syndrome, and Angelman syndrome [14]. It is a comparatively simple organism, with its genome consisting of four chromosomes encoding approximately 14,000 genes [15, 16]. On the other hand, there are 20,000 protein and RNA- coding genes in the human genome [17]. Among 59 of the human neurological genes examined, 38 have orthologs in the Drosophila genome [18]. A single Drosophila gene may serve the same function as multiple related genes of mammals, thus decreasing the redundancy seen in other vertebrate models. Although flies and humans are distinctly different from each other, many molecular processes are conserved between them. The advantage of studying neurobehavioral disorders in Drosophila is the presence of genes that are similar to human genes for normal cognitive functions [19, 20]. The Drosophila model exhibits complex behaviors relevant to humans, including courtship [21, 22], circadian rhythms [23], learning and memory [24], aggression [25], grooming [26], and open field exploration [27]. It is also an attractive model because of its quicker generation time, large number of progeny for better selection, and easy maintenance.

There are differences between pregnant fruit flies and pregnant women. Fruit flies deliver their embryos, after which the development of embryo to fruit fly takes place outside, whereas embryo-to-infant development takes place inside the uterus in women. Therefore, the fruit fly is considered a good model to study the transition between stages, i.e., embryo; 1st, 2nd, and 3rd instar larva; and pupa to fly. We have previously reported that environmental risk factors cause neurobehavioral changes in Drosophila. In fruit flies, we observed that exposure to bisphenol A (BPA) affects behavior [28], and that methylmercury exposure inhibits alcohol dehydrogenase [29] and affects sexual functions [30].

In this study, we have examined the effects of TI to female flies on the development of larvae and the behavior of the first generation of flies. Climbing or negative geotaxis is an innate behavior of the fruit fly. There has been considerable interest in using this simple behavior to gain insight into the alterations in brain function [31]. Therefore, we compared negative geotaxis in TI and control offspring flies to study whether TI to female flies induces behavior abnormalities in the offspring. We also studied the effects of TI on social interaction among the offspring flies by measuring the distance between a fly and its nearest neighbor in a social space assay, as described previously [28, 32].

Materials and methods

Drosophila melanogaster

Wild-type Oregon-R Drosophila melanogaster stocks were maintained at 250 C on a standard cornmeal diet (Jazz-mix Drosophila food, Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA) under 12 h:12 h light and dark cycle.

Traumatic injury to female flies

As shown in Fig. 1, 10-ml polypropylene tubes (Genesee Scientific; catalogue number 21-392) (Fig. 1A, #1) were cut below the cap (Fig. 1A, #2), and a cotton plug (Fig. 1A, #3) was placed. The tube was marked at 2 ml and 8 ml (Fig. 1A, #4) Twenty female flies (6–8 days old) were separated from male flies just before they were transferred to these tubes. The flies were brought down to the 2-ml mark on the tubes by tapping the tube and pushing down the cotton plug (Fig. 1A, #5). We induced TI to female flies following the method of Katenberger [33]. As described in Fig. 1B, C, the top end of the tube was slid in the spring to the 8-ml mark, and then the spring was released to hit the platform jack to induce TI to the flies.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Procedure of inducing traumatic injury in flies. A plastic tube (10 ml) was cut below the cap, and it was marked at 2 ml and 8 ml. The flies were transferred into the tube and pushed down to the 2-ml mark by a cotton plug (A). The plastic tube was then inserted into a spring up to the 8-ml mark (B). As shown C, the spring was then lifted by holding the spring at its 12-cm mark so that the spring could touch the stand. After that, the spring was released to hit the platform jack, which had a 2-cm-thick wooden plate

The female flies were transferred to food vials at 7 pm immediately after TI. At the same time, 20 control flies (without TI) of the same age were also transferred to food vials. The next day at 7 am, the flies were removed, and the development of larvae was studied.

In this report, larvae, pupae and offspring flies that emerged from maternal flies after TI are identified as TI-larvae, TI-pupae and TI flies. Similarly, larvae, pupae and flies obtained from control female flies are identified as C-larvae, C-pupae and C-flies.

Handling of larvae

When larvae were 4 days old (3rd instar larvae), 20% sucrose solution was added to the food vials, and the floating larvae were removed after 5 min. The larvae were placed on glass slides and kept in a refrigerator. Photographs of the larvae along with a scale were taken.

Measurement of length of larvae

The changes in larval length in Drosophila may indicate the changes in development. We measured the length of larvae using ImageJ NIH software (http://imagej.nih.gov/ij/). In brief, a file was opened in ImageJ. A straight line was selected, and a line (1-cm- long) on the scale was drawn, followed by clicking ‘analyze’ and ‘measure’. Next, ‘set scale’ was clicked; followed by clicking ‘change distance’ to 1 cm (do not change distance in pixels, and ‘aspect ratio’), and then ‘OK’ was clicked. Then ‘analyze’ followed by ‘measure’ was clicked. This technique produces a 1-cm-length measuring tool. Afterwards, a line (straight or free-hand line) was drawn on the larval length, and ‘measure’ was clicked each time, yielding the length of larvae in cm.

Time taken for origination of flies from pupae

The control and TI flies were placed in the food vials to lay eggs for 12 h from 7 pm to 7 am the next day. The flies were removed at 7 am, and pupae-to-fly conversion, i.e., eclosion of flies was counted each morning.

Negative geotaxis

In one set of experiment, 20 flies of the first generation (6–8 days old) were transferred to a Drosophila vial (narrow), and another vial was placed over it. These two vials were then joined with tape. To count the number of flies crossing 3/4th of the distance in the two vials, a line was drawn at the ¾th distance (14.5 cm) on the two vials. The tubes were tapped five times, and movement of the flies was video-recorded. The number of flies crossing the 14.5-cm mark was counted. One more set of experiments with different group of flies was conducted in a similar manner.

Social interaction

Social interaction was determined in male and female flies by using the social space assay, as we described previously [28]. The plastic chamber used for this assay had a triangle of 16 cm base and 17 cm height, which was covered from both sides by square plastic plates (18 cm × 18 cm). In brief, the first generation of flies (6–8 days old) from the control (n = 20) and TI groups (n = 20) were separated according to their gender, and transferred to the chamber. After the flies spent 40 min exploring and settling in the chamber, a digital image of the chamber with the flies was taken with a camera. Image J was then used to process the image into an 8-bit binary image. The binary image was then imported to the Lispix program (NIH image analysis software—http://www.nist.gov/lispix/) to calculate the distance of the fly to its nearest neighboring fly.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed for larval size by Student t test, and for negative geotaxis and social interaction (distance of the fly to its nearest neighbor fly) by one-way ANOVA using GraphPad Prism 10.

Results

Effect of traumatic injury on survival rate of female flies

It was observed that 20–30% of traumatically injured female flies died. Therefore, the number of maternal flies after TI was always less than the number of the control maternal flies. However, we used the same number of control and TI offspring flies in each assay.

Traumatic injury affects larval length

The larvae (4 days old) were collected and their length was measured as described in Methods section. TI to female flies resulted in a significant increase (p = 0.0365) in larval length (0.2478 ± 0.026 cm, mean ± SD, N = 20) as compared to that in control flies (0.2147 ± 0.31 cm, mean ± SD, N = 20) (Fig. 2). This data suggests that the development of the larvae gets accelerated in the TI group.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Effect of traumatic injury to female flies on the length of larvae. The length of larvae in the TI and control groups was measured. Twenty larvae in each group were used to measure the length of larvae. There was a significant increase in the larvae length of TI-larvae (larvae obtained from traumatically injured female flies) as compared to control larvae (larvae obtained from female flies without TI). The data were analyzed with Student t-test

Traumatic injury in female flies causes early metamorphosis from pupa to fly

Metamorphosis of flies, i.e., pupa to fly conversion was studied in TI and control flies. In the TI female flies group, pupae formation was earlier by a day and the metamorphosis started on the 12th day after TI to female flies (30% of the first generation of flies were observed) and 100% of flies were seen by the 13th day. On the other hand, no first generation of flies were seen on the 12th day in the control female flies group. Metamorphosis in control flies was observed on the 13th and 14th days. Our results showed that TI to the female flies causes a shift in the timing of metamorphosis (Fig. 3). We also observed that total number of offspring flies in TI group were slightly less as compared to control group.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Origination of flies from traumatic injury-pupae (pupae obtained from traumatically injured female flies) and control- pupae (pupae obtained from female flies without TI). TI caused a shift in the timing of metamorphosis. In the TI group, flies started emerging 1 day earlier than flies in the control group. Three sets of independent experiments were done in each group

Effect of traumatic injury on negative geotaxis in first generation of flies

The movement of flies in an upward direction (climbing) is called negative geotaxis. The negative geotaxis assay is important for studying behavioral changes in fruit flies. TI in female flies affected the negative geotaxis in the first generation of both male and female flies (Fig. 4). A significant reduction in negative geotaxis was observed in both TI male (p = 0.0021) and TI female flies (p = 0.0426) compared to their respective control groups. Twenty male flies and 20 female flies in each control and TI group were studied. In male flies, 6.14 ± 2.4 TI flies (mean ± SD) crossed the ¾th mark as compared to 11.4 ± 2.7 (mean ± SD) flies in the control group. In female flies, 1.57 ± 0.78 TI flies (mean ± SD) crossed the ¾th mark as compared to 3.28 ± 1.1 (mean ± SD) flies in the control group. It was also observed that negative geotaxis was significantly greater in male flies than in female flies in both the TI (p = 0.0152) and the control group (p = 0.0007).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effect of TI to female flies on negative geotaxis in first generation of flies. Negative geotaxis was significantly reduced in both male and female flies that were born after TI to female flies. C, control; TI, traumatic injury. The data were analyzed with one-way ANOVA by using the Sidak multiple comparison test. Two independent sets of experiments were done

Abnormal social Interactions in first generation of male and female flies after traumatic injury to female flies

The distance between an individual fly and its closest neighbor has been used as a measure of social interaction within the group, as previously described [30, 34]. TI in female flies caused a significant effect on the distance maintained between the male and female flies (first generation) in a social group (Fig. 5). The first generation of male and female flies that originated from traumatically injured female flies was significantly closer in distance than in control flies (male flies, p = 0.0232; female flies, p = 0.049). Two sets of independent experiments with different flies were conducted in each group.

Fig. 5
figure 5

TI decreases the distance between a Drosophila fly and its closest neighbor in a social setting. The data are shown as box and whisker plots of the distance to the closest neighbor in the chamber, with the box representing the first quartile (25th percent) and the 3rd quartile (75th percent), the line in the box representing the median, and the Tukey whiskers excluding the outliers. These data were obtained from two independent repeats of 20 flies per assay. Data were analyzed by one-way ANOVA using the Sidak multiple comparison test

Discussion

It has been suggested that trauma complicates approximately 1 in 12 pregnancies [34], and it is the leading non-obstetric cause of maternal death [35]. Trauma also has fetal implications and has been reported to increase the incidence of spontaneous abortion, preterm premature rupture of membranes, preterm birth, uterine rupture, cesarean delivery, placental abruption and stillbirth [36,37,38,39,40]. The rate of fetal death from maternal trauma is 2.3 per 100,000 live births [39], with placental abruption as a major contributing factor [41]. It is reported that 1 in 3 pregnant women admitted for trauma to the hospital delivers during her hospitalization [42]. While pregnancy per se does not appear to increase morbidity or mortality due to trauma, the presence of a gravid uterus alters the pattern of injury [41].

Drosophila melanogaster has been used as an animal model for traumatic brain injury [33]. In this study, we used this model to study how TI in pregnant female flies affects the offspring. We had anticipated that TI to female flies would lead to a decrease in larval length. However, to our surprise, we observed an increase in TI-larval length. TI-larvae also had faster pupae formation and metamorphosis (pupa to fly conversion) as compared to control larvae. Because pupae formation was faster in TI-larvae, we also observed a shift in metamorphosis, i.e., early metamorphosis by at least 1 day in TI-pupae than in the C-pupae. The reason for this rapid growth may be that TI in maternal flies led to faster delivery of embryos or that inflammation was increased as compared to control flies.

We also studied whether the behavior of offspring flies born after TI to female flies would be affected. Our results indicate that TI to female flies resulted in decreased negative geotaxis in both male and female flies, suggesting that TI in pregnant flies may also affect the behavior of the offspring. Not only was the negative geotaxis decreased but the distance between the nearest neighbors in flies was also decreased in the first generation of both male and female flies after TI to female flies as compared to control group without TI, suggesting that TI in flies leads to impaired social interaction in the offspring. We are not yet certain whether negative geotaxis has any direct link to social interaction, i.e., altered nearest neighbor distance of the flies. Since both these parameters are on behavioral changes in fruit fly, there is a possibility that alterations in these assays (negative geotaxis and social space assay) caused by TI to female flies may have some association.

One of the core features of autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders such as fragile X and Angelman’s syndromes is the inability of the individual to interact socially with other individuals [43,44,45], which is a diagnostic criterion in autism. It is also a common feature in studies using mouse models for autism [46]. Social behavior is the ability of conspecifics to interact, leading to changes in the subsequent behaviors of the individual [47,48,49]. In a social setting, the individual maintains a personal space or distance from another individual (personal space boundary), but also a spatial proximity to another individual for effective communication [50]. This social space or the space between two individuals of the same species is seen in most animals such as birds, fish, or locusts. When placed in a social setting, flies tend to arrange themselves uniformly rather than in aggregates or randomly [51, 52], and this social interaction in the group leads to learning of higher behaviors from their conspecifics [47, 52, 53]. Studies have shown that social isolation in Drosophila reduces the fiber number in the mushroom bodies, the functional equivalent of the hippocampus [54]; increases aggression [55,56,57]; and shortens the life span [57]. When placed in settings where they can freely interact with other flies, Drosophila flies usually maintain a distance of about two-body lengths (1–5 mm) among themselves, similar to other animals [32, 52]. This allows for the flies to orient themselves to interact with each other. Thus, interaction between flies follows a non-random pattern [58]. In our study, inter-fly distance was used as a measure for social interaction [59,60,61,62]. We have previously reported a decrease in the inter-fly distance when the flies exposed to BPA were placed in a social setting [28]. This decrease may be due to aberrant social interaction, in which the flies do not maintain the ideal balance of attraction/repulsion and interact inappropriately with each other. It is quite possible that children born after TI may also exhibit lack of social interaction features as observed in autism and fragile X syndrome.

Conclusion

In female fruit flies, TI affected the larval length and metamorphosis (pupa to fly conversion), thus affecting the development of offspring. In addition, the behavior of flies born after TI to maternal female flies was also affected as assessed by negative geotaxis and social interaction (distance between the fly and its nearest neighbor) assays. These results suggest that TI in female flies causes developmental abnormalities in offspring, which results in abnormal behavioral functions of adult flies. Therefore, TI during pregnancy in human may play a major role in affecting the development and behavior of children.

Data analysis

Data were analyzed for larval size by Student t-test, and for negative geotaxis and social interaction (distance of the fly to its nearest neighbor fly) by one-way ANOVA using GraphPad Prism 10. All the comparisons were tested and only significant comparisons are labelled.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ASD:

autism spectrum disorder

BPA:

Bisphenol A

CDC:

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention

TI:

traumatic injury

References

  1. Boyle CA, Boulet S, Schieve LA, Cohen RA, Blumberg SJ, Yeargin-Allsopp M, et al. Trends in the prevalence of developmental disabilities in US children 1997–2008. Pediatrics. 2011;127:1034–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Luke L, Clare IC, Ring H, Redley M, Watson P. Decision-making difficulties experienced by adults with autism spectrum conditions. Autism. 2012;16:612–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. El-Kady D, Gilbert WM, Anderson J, et al. Trauma during pregnancy: an analysis of maternal and fetal outcomes in a large population. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2004;190:1661–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Peckham CH, King RW. A study of intercurrent conditions observed during pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1963;87:609–24.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Agran PF, Dunkle DE, Winn DG, et al. Fetal death in motor vehicle accidents. Ann Emerg Med. 1987;16:1355–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Drost TF, Rosemurgy AS, Sherman HF, et al. Major trauma in pregnant women: maternal/fetal outcome. J Trauma Injury Infect Crit Care. 1990;30:574–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Esposito TJ, Gens DR, Smith LG, et al. Evaluation of blunt abdominal trauma occurring during pregnancy. J Trauma Injury Infect Crit Care. 1989;29:1628–32.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Goodwin TM, Breen MT. Pregnancy outcome and fetomaternal hemorrhage after noncatastrophic trauma. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1990;162:665–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lavin JP Jr, Polsky SS. Abdominal trauma during pregnancy. Clin Perinatol. 1983;10:423–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Shaw OEF, Yager JY. Preventing childhood and lifelong disability: maternal dietary supplementation for perinatal brain injury. Pharmacol Res. 2019;139:228–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Vexler ZS, Ferriero DM. Molecular and biochemical mechanisms of perinatal brain injury. Semin Neonatol. 2001;6(2):99–108.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Novak I, Hines M, Goldsmith S, Barclay R. Clinical prognostic messages from a systematic review on cerebral palsy. Pediatrics. 2012;130(5):e1285–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Shevell MI, Dagenais L, Hall N. The relationship of cerebral palsy subtype and functional motor impairment: a population-based study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2009;51(11):872–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Mackay TF, Anholt RR. Of flies and man: Drosophila as a model for human complex traits. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2006;7:339–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Adams MD, Celniker SE, Holt RA, Evans CA, Gocayne JD, Amanatides PG, et al. The genome sequence of Drosophila melanogaster. Science. 2000;287:2185–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Celniker SE, Rubin GM. The Drosophila melanogaster genome. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2003;4:89–117.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Moraes F, Góes A. A decade of human genome project conclusion: scientific diffusion about our genome knowledge. Biochem Mol Biol Educ. 2016;44:215–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Rubin GM, Yandell MD, Wortman JR, Gabor Miklos GL, Nelson CR, Hariharan IK, et al. Comparative genomics of the eukaryotes. Science. 2000;287:2204–15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Inlow JK, Restifo LL. Molecular and comparative genetics of mental retardation. Genetics. 2004;166:835–81.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Restifo LL. Mental retardation genes in Drosophila: new approaches to under-standing and treating developmental brain disorders. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2005;11:286–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Coen P, Clemens J, Weinstein AJ, Pacheco DA, Deng Y, Murthy M. Dynamic sensory cues shape song structure in Drosophila. Nature. 2014;507:233–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Dickson BJ. Wired for sex: the neurobiology of Drosophila mating decisions. Science. 2008;322:904–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Konopka RJ, Benzer S. Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1971;68:2112–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Tully T, Preat T, Boynton SC, Del Vecchio M. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila. Cell. 1994;79:35–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Chen S, Lee AY, Bowens NM, Huber R, Kravitz EA. Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:5664–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Tauber JM, Vanlandingham PA, Zhang B. Elevated levels of the vesicular monoamine transporter and a novel repetitive behavior in the Drosophila model of fragile X syndrome. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e27100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Burns JG, Svetec N, Rowe L, Mery F, Dolan MJ, Boyce WT, et al. Gene-environment interplay in Drosophila melanogaster: chronic food deprivation in early life affects adult exploratory and fitness traits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(Suppl 2):17239–44.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Kaur K, Simon AF, Chauhan V, Chauhan A. Effect of bisphenol A on Drosophila melanogaster behavior—a new model for the studies on neurodevelopmental disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2015;284:77–84.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Chauhan V, Chauhan A. Effects of methylmercury and alcohol exposure in Drosophila melanogaster: potential risks in neurodevelopmental disorders. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2016;51:36–41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Chauhan V, Srikumar S, Aamer S, Pandareesh MD, Chauhan A. Methylmercury exposure induces sexual dysfunction in male and female Drosophila melanogaster. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14(10):E1108.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Willenbrink AM, Gronauer MK, Toebben LF, Kick DR, Wells M, Zhang B. The Hillary Climber trumps manual testing: an automatic system for studying Drosophila climbing. J Neurogenet. 2016;30:205–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Simon AF, Chou MT, Salazar ED, Nicholson T, Saini N, Metchev S, et al. A simple assay to study social behavior in Drosophila: measurement of social space within a group. Genes Brain Behav. 2012;11:243–52.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Katzenberger RJ, Loewen CA, Wassarman DR, Petersen AJ, Ganetzky B, Wassarman DA. A Drosophila model of closed head traumatic brain injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2013;110(44):E4152–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Hill CC, Pickinpaugh J. Trauma and surgical emergencies in the obstetric patient. Surg Clin North Am. 2008;88:421–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Fildes J, Reed L, Jones N, Martin M, Barrett J. Trauma: the leading cause of maternal death. J Trauma. 1992;32:643–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Pak LL, Reece EA, Chan L. Is adverse pregnancy outcome predictable after blunt abdominal trauma? Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1998;179:1140–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Schiff MA, Holt VL. Pregnancy outcomes following hospitalization for motor vehicle crashes in Washington state from 1989 to 2001. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161:503–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Weiss HB, Songer TJ, Fabio A. Fetal deaths related to maternal injury. JAMA. 2001;286:1863–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Pearlman MD, Tintinallli JE, Lorenz RP. A prospective controlled study of outcome after trauma during pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1990;162:1502–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Schiff MA, Holt VL, Daling JR. Maternal and infant outcomes after injury during pregnancy in Washington state from 1989 to 1997. J Trauma. 2002;53:939–45.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Shah KH, Simons RK, Holbrook T, Fortlage D, Winchell RJ, Hoyt DB. Trauma in pregnancy: maternal and fetal outcomes. J Trauma. 1998;45:83–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kuo C, Jamieson DJ, McPheeters ML, Meikle SF, Posner SF. Injury hospitalizations of pregnant women in the United States, 2002. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2007;196(161):e1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Wing L, Gould J. Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnor-malities in children: epidemiology and classification. J Autism Dev Disord. 1979;9:11–29.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Kaufmann WE, Cortell R, Kau AS, Bukelis I, Tierney E, Gray RM, et al. Autism spectrum disorder in fragile X syndrome: communication, social interaction, and specific behaviors. Am J Med Genet A. 2004;129A:225–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Wadge H, Brewer R, Bird G, Toni I, Stolk A. Communicative misalignment in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Cortex. 2019;115:15–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Jamain S, Radyushkin K, Hammerschmidt K, Granon S, Boretius S, Varo-queaux F, et al. Reduced social interaction and ultrasonic communication in a mouse model of monogenic heritable autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:1710–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Sokolowski MB. Social interactions in simple model systems. Neuron. 2010;65:780–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Novelli D, Drury J, Reicher S. Come together: two studies concerning the impact of group relations on personal space. Br J Soc Psychol. 2010;49:223–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Kohn NR, Reaume CJ, Moreno C, Burns JG, Sokolowski MB, Mery F. Social environment influences performance in a cognitive task in natural variants of the foraging gene. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e81272.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Sommer R. Studies in personal space. Sociometry. 1959;22:247–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Sexton OJ, Stalker HD. Spacing patterns of female Drosophila paramelanica. Anim Behav. 1961;9:77–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Billeter JC, Jagadeesh S, Stepek N, Azanchi R, Levine JD. Drosophila melanogaster females change mating behaviour and offspring production based on social context. Proc Biol Sci. 2012;279:2417–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Krupp JJ, Kent C, Billeter JC, Azanchi R, So AK, Schonfeld JA, et al. Social experience modifies pheromone expression and mating behavior in male Drosophila melanogaster. Curr Biol. 2008;18:1373–83.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Technau GM. Fiber number in the mushroom bodies of adult Drosophila melanogaster depends on age, sex and experience. J Neurogenet. 2007;21:183–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Hoffmann AA. The influence of age and experience with conspecifics on territorial behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. J Insect Behav. 1990;3:1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Zhou C, Rao Y, Rao Y. A subset of octopaminergic neurons are important for Drosophila aggression. Nat Neurosci. 2008;11:1059–67.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Ruan H, Wu CF. Social interaction-mediated lifespan extension of Drosophila Cu/Zn superoxide dismutase mutants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:7506–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  58. Schneider J, Dickinson MH, Levine JD. Social structures depend on innate determinants and chemosensory processing in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(Suppl 2):17174–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Burg ED, Langan ST, Nash HA. Drosophila social clustering is disrupted by anesthetics and in narrow abdomen ion channel mutants. Genes Brain Behav. 2013;12:338–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Hahn N, Geurten B, Gurvich A, Piepenbrock D, Kastner A, Zanini D, et al. Monogenic heritable autism gene neuroligin impacts Drosophila social behavior. Behav Brain Res. 2013;252:450–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Bolduc FV, Valente D, Nguyen AT, Mitra PP, Tully T. An assay for social interaction in Drosophila Fragile X mutants. Fly (Austin). 2010;4:216–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Schneider J, Atallah J, Levine JD. One, two, and many—a perspective on what groups of Drosophila melanogaster can tell us about social dynamics. Adv Genet. 2012;77:59–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported in part by funds from the New York State Office for People with Developmental Disabilities and the Autism Research Institute.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

VC planned the study, performed the experiments, analyzed data and wrote the manuscript. AC helped in planning the study, data analysis and manuscript preparation. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ved Chauhan.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable. The work is on Drosophila melanogaster, which does not require approval and consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chauhan, V., Chauhan, A. Traumatic injury in female Drosophila melanogaster affects the development and induces behavioral abnormalities in the offspring. Behav Brain Funct 15, 11 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-019-0163-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-019-0163-1

Keywords