Skip to main content

Maternal depression is associated with altered functional connectivity between neural circuits related to visual, auditory, and cognitive processing during stories listening in preschoolers

Abstract

Background

Maternal depression can influence the early activity of a mother reading stories to a young child, as depressed mothers are less likely to read to their children. Here, maternal depression association to neurobiological circuitry of narrative comprehension, visualization, and executive functions during stories listening was examined in 21 4-year-old girls and their mothers. Maternal depression scores were collected from the mothers, and functional MRI during stories listening was collected from the children.

Results

Increased maternal depression was related to decreased functional connectivity between visualization and auditory regions and increased connectivity between the right visual cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the children.

Conclusions

This study highlights the need to monitor maternal depression and provide interventions to ensure positive linguistic outcomes in children.

Key points

  • Previous research has indicated that maternal depression plays a role in childhood development (i.e., in executive functions) and that depressed mothers speak differently to their children.

  • The current study explored the neurobiological underpinnings of maternal depression and child language development.

  • Maternal depression was related to decreased functional connections between visualization and auditory regions and increased connectivity in frontal and visual regions in the children during stories listening.

  • Maternal depression should be observed during child-wellness office visits so that interventions such as dialogic reading could be implemented to ensure cognitively stimulating interactions between mother and child.

Background

Child language development: critical period and components

Early childhood is a critical period for development. The young brain learns the sounds and meaning of language. Extensive synaptogenesis and pruning occurs from birth until the age of 7 years, when children learn the phonological and grammatical characteristics of the language they are exposed to [1], and is ongoing into adolescence in the frontal cortices, especially in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [2]. Young children use a set of innate human-specific skills, particularly auditory word recognition and syntactic and semantic processing, when listening to linguistic stimulation [3]. As found by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), these linguistic abilities rely on phonological processing supported by the superior temporal gyrus (STG, BA 22) and the angular gyrus (AG, BA 40, 39), as well as semantics and syntax engaging the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, BA 44, 45). The ability to comprehend language also relies on abilities that are not specific to language, such as executive functions, supported by frontal regions (BA 10, 6, 46, 9, 8). Each of these regions proves instrumental in the development and usage of the mentioned linguistic functions [4].

An additional cognitive ability essential for language processing is visualization [5]. Visualization is the ability to create a visual representation of verbal stimulation and has been found to be related to improved narrative comprehension [6] and reading comprehension [7]. The role of BA 17, 18 in visual imagery or visualization has been previously determined (see [8]). Increased literacy environment (i.e., more books in the household, more hours read to the child) in children ages 3–5 years is related to increased activation in visual processing regions during stories listening [6]. The presumption was that children with increased environmental stimulation are able to imagine the stories better by relying on visual processing regions. These findings were supported by our additional findings in the same age population, 4-year-old children who listened to stories while their future reading network was defined based on previous studies [9]. We demonstrated that stories listening in 4-year-old children “fit into” their future reading network and suggested that recruitment of regions related to visual imagery during the process of listening to stories will support future reading acquisition.

Maternal depression and child language development

One of the most influential environmental components in a child’s life is his or her primary caregiver, typically the mother. Mother–child interaction may be affected by the mental state of the mother [10]. Maternal depression is manifested through clinically elevated depressive symptoms and is prevalent in low-income mothers. Ammerman and colleagues reported that 44.3% of 806 mothers exceeded a clinical cutoff for a depression screening on at least one of two administrations during the 1st-year postpartum Ammerman, Putnam [11]. An examination of the relationship between depression and parenting indicated decreased nurturing and less involvement with the child in depressed compared to non-depressed mothers using the HOME Inventory [12].

Depression in mothers may be associated with an absence of consistent environment and a lack of responsiveness to the child Sohr-Preston [10]. The authors suggested that depressed mothers tend to respond negatively to their children and interact with them less, which may lead to decreased cognitive and language outcomes for the child. These mothers tend to use less infant-directed speech, less facial interaction, and less use of “motherese”—a way of speaking that includes higher tones and exaggerated prosody that triggers an infant’s attention [13,14,15,16]. Interestingly, such interaction was previously related to the activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) [2, 17]. Despite the accumulated information of the negative effect of maternal depression levels on interaction with the child and the cognitive and linguistic outcomes, the impact of maternal depression on the developing neural circuitry of the child when listening to stories has not been tested. Revealing the impact of maternal depression on these neurobiological circuits, especially those related to imagination and visualization, may lead to a better understanding of the mechanisms related to decreased linguistic outcomes.

In this study, we sought to determine the neurobiological relationship between maternal depression and child language development, specifically focusing on visualization abilities while listening to stories. fMRI data acquired while 4-year-old girls listened to stories was tested for association with the depression severity of their mothers. Based on several studies demonstrating decreased functional connections between the visual imagery network and cognitive control network or regions in conditions with less engagement vs those with greater engagement [18, 19], we hypothesized that decreased functional connections between the visual processing and cognitive control regions would be found. More specifically we hypothesized that increased maternal depression would be associated with decreased functional connections between visual processing regions and auditory/language regions due to less efficient stories-listening processes in the children of depressed mothers. We also suggested greater functional connections between visual processing regions and cognitive control areas, specifically the DLPFC (per [2, 17]) due to non-automatic visualization processes while listening to stories in girls with mothers exhibiting greater to severe depressive symptoms.

Methods

Participants

Children (N = 21, mean age: 48 months, all right-handed girls) and their mothers (N = 21, mean age: 21.57 years) participated in the current study. All children were within the normal range of nonverbal IQ, none had a history of neurological or emotional disorders or attention difficulties, and all were native monolingual English speakers with no contraindications to MRI. Almost half of mothers (47.5%) had less than a college education at the time of the study. We chose to include only girls and their mothers in this study to avoid the confounding influence of sexual dimorphism on child brain-language development and based on previous experience showing greater MRI success rates in girls versus boys in the 4-years-old age group [20, 21]. Written informed consent was obtained from mothers, with verbal assent from the children. Participants were recruited from a longitudinal study of injury prevention in young children of low income, first-time mothers enrolled in a home visiting program [22]. The study received Institutional Review Board approval (CHIP study, Protocol number: 2010-2537).

Behavioral measures

The performance and verbal subtests from the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI) [23] were administered to verify normal nonverbal and verbal IQ, respectively. Verbal ability was assessed by instructing the child to point at the correct picture corresponding to the given verbal cue. Nonverbal ability was assessed by instructing the child to point at the missing part of an incomplete shape. The average of the tests from the WPPSI was 100 with a standard deviation of 15.

To exclude linguistic deficits, phonological awareness was assessed using the Elision subtest from the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP) [24]. In this test, the child was requested to omit a sound from an orally given word. The average of the CTOPP test was a scaled score of 10 with standard deviation of 3.

Reading ability of the mothers was assessed by the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE), specifically the sight word efficiency and phonetic decoding efficiency subtests [25]. The mothers’ depression levels were assessed using the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) [26]. The BDI-II was administered at 12 months postpartum, 24 months postpartum, and at the study visit. This self-report assessment consists of 21 items to assess depression symptoms. A score of 0–13 indicates minimal depression, 14–19 indicates mild depression, 20–28 indicates moderate depression, and 29–63 indicates severe depression [26]. Averages, minimum and maximum values, as well as skewness and kurtosis of the behavioral scores were calculated. To characterize the persistent nature of exposure of children to maternal depression, we calculated the total of each of the three administrations of the BDI–II. This approach has been used to examine the impact of persistent maternal depression on child neurocognitive processes [27].

MRI procedure

Stories-listening task

For the stories-listening fMRI task, a total of five stories were presented in a female voice, one for each 30-s task period (transcripts of a story used can be found in [4] and the audio of stories can be downloaded from https://irc.cchmc.org/software/pedaudio.php). The stories presented were 9, 10, or 11 sentences in length and contained varying syntactic structures and vocabulary appropriate for the preschool level. The sentences were randomized across scans. For contrast with the stories, backwards speech was presented with frequencies of 200–400 Hz for 30-s intervals between each story as a control for sub-lexical auditory processing in the fMRI analysis stage.

MRI acquisition and data preparation

The children were acclimated and desensitized to condition them for comfort inside the MRI scanner (see [28, 29] for details). Communication was established between the child and study coordinator through headphones equipped with a built-in microphone. Children were asked to listen to the stories they heard through the headphones. Verbal communication and positive reinforcement were maintained with the child throughout the scan. Scanning was terminated immediately if the child did not wish to continue. Children were not sedated and were awake during the entire duration of the MRI scan, as monitored through the use of an MRI-compatible eye-tracking system (Real-eye, Arrington Research, Inc., Scottsdale, AZ).

MRI scans of the children were acquired using a 3T Philips Achieva MRI system (Philips Medical Systems, Best, The Netherlands) equipped with an Avotec audiovisual system and the eye-tracking system. MRI acquisition included a 3D anatomical brain image for registration of functional data, as well as a fMRI scan during stimulation with a stories-listening task as described here and elsewhere [4, 30]. For fMRI, a time-series of 165 blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD)-weighted scans with the following parameters: TR/TE = 2000/38 ms; BW = 125 kHz; FOV = 25.6 × 25.6 cm; matrix = 64 × 64; slice thickness = 5 mm covering the entire brain with 38 slices in the axial plane were acquired with voxel size 3.75 × 3.75 × 5 mm at 2-s intervals during a single run of the stories-listening task.

Data processing was performed using FSL software (fMRI-Brain Software, version 6, Library, Oxford, UK) [31]. Data were processed to correct images for head motion during the time series by using the MCFLIRT routine in FSL. Slice-timing correction, spatial smoothing, and intensity normalization were all applied using the FEAT (fMRI Expert Analysis Tool, version 6.00) modality of FSL for our BOLD analyses, followed by magnetic-field map correction, normalization of all images to the Montreal Neurological (MNI) space, a spatial smoothing with an 8-mm full width at half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel, and registration to the structural 3D images. In addition, a threshold for excessive motion (> 2 mm) was determined. We used a mutual information cost function for rejecting motion-corrupted frames of fMRI data, as previously described [32]. All data met the criterion of median voxel displacement < 2 mm in the center of the brain.

Following the described preprocessing steps, functional image time-series data were entered into functional connectivity analysis.

Functional connectivity analysis

To determine the correlation between maternal depression and the functional connectivity of the child’s processing of imagery, we picked visual processing regions already described in previous studies as participating in imagery processes [33] as the seeds for this analysis. Using the Brodmann area (BA) atlas implemented in the CONN toolbox (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn/; [34]), we focused on both bilateral BA 17 and 18 as our seeds-of-interest and correlated the functional connectivity between these seeds and all BAs in the brain with maternal depression scores.

Once the BOLD signal from the background speech was subtracted from the BOLD data from the stories-listening task, analysis of functional connectivity during the stories task was carried out using the CONN toolbox [34]. Normalized bias-corrected T1 images were generated in SPM (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) and segmented into gray matter, white matter, and cerebral spinal fluid (CSF). The principle eigenvariate of the BOLD time-courses from white matter and CSF, as well as the six motion-correction parameters were included as regressors-of-no-interest and were detrended from the fMRI time-series data. The data were then band-pass filtered between 0.008 and 0.2 Hz (as recommended [35]), and only data within this range were included in the connectivity analysis in the next step. We then conducted a functional connectivity analysis between seeds placed in the child’s visual processing brain regions and other regions in the brain during stories listening. Finally, we tested these connections for associations with maternal depression level. Data was corrected for multiple comparisons for each ROI.

Results

Behavioral measures

Performance and verbal abilities were average to above average in all of the children. Maternal reading ability was in the average to below average range. Maternal depression level was in a mild range. There were 13 mothers with a minimal depression, three with mild depression, two with a moderate depression, and three with severe depression. See Table 1 for details.

Table 1 Behavioral measures of children and their mothers

Neuroimaging measures

Functional connectivity of visual processing regions and other regions in the brain during stories listening

Overall, we found positive functional connectivity during the stories-listening task between the child’s left visual cortex (BA 17, BA 18) and regions related to vision (BA 7, 17–19, 30), reward and emotional processing (BA 11, left BA 38), cognitive control (10, 31, left BA 46), word reading (right BA 39), and language (right BA 37). Additionally, positive functional connectivity during the stories task was found between the right visual cortex (BA 17, BA 18) and other visual regions (BA 17–19, 30), language (right BA 37), emotional processing (right BA 38, BA 29, right BA 23), cognitive control (BA 10, 31, left BA 46), reward processing (BA 11), word reading (right BA 39), and memory (right BA 27) (Fig. 1 and listed in Table 2; P < 0.05, FDR corrected).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Visual seeds chosen for the analysis; Brodmann areas 17 and 18

Table 2 Target regions exhibiting increased functional connectivity to visual regions during stories listening

Correlations between BDI–II scores and functional connectivity during stories listening in the selected ROIs

Negative correlations between BDI scores and functional connectivity when listening to stories were observed between visual (left BA 17) and auditory (right BA 42) and language (right BA 43) regions; i.e., higher depression levels were related to decreased functional connectivity between visual and auditory and language regions. Additionally, a positive correlation was found between BDI scores and functional connectivity during the stories-listening task between visual (right BA 18) and cognitive control (right BA 46) regions; i.e., higher depression levels were associated with increased functional connections between visual and cognitive control regions. Overall, greater depression level was related to a decreased association between visual and auditory regions and increased reliance of cognitive control for visualization (see Table 3 and Figs. 2, 3, 4, 5, 6 and 7).

Table 3 Functional connectivity between visual regions during stories listening and maternal depression score
Fig. 2
figure 2

Left, right, and superior views of seed-to-region of interest (ROI) functional connectivity of left Brodmann area (BA) 17. Left BA 17 seed is shown in gray. Significant target ROIs are shown in red. Size of ROIs corresponds to effect size. L left, R right

Fig. 3
figure 3

Left, right, and superior views of seed-to-region of interest (ROI) functional connectivity of left Brodmann area (BA) 18. Left BA 18 seed is shown in gray. Significant target ROIs are shown in red. Size of ROIs corresponds to effect size. L left, R right

Fig. 4
figure 4

Left, right, and superior views of seed-to-region of interest (ROI) functional connectivity of right Brodmann area (BA) 17. Right BA 17 seed is shown in gray. Significant target ROIs are shown in red. Size of ROIs corresponds to effect size. L left, R right

Fig. 5
figure 5

Left, right, and superior views of seed-to-region of interest (ROI) functional connectivity of right Brodmann area (BA) 18. Right BA 18 seed is shown in gray. Significant target ROIs are shown in red. Size of ROIs corresponds to effect size. L left; R right

Fig. 6
figure 6

Left and superior views of the decreased functional connectivity between the left primary visual cortex [Brodmann area (BA) 17, gray] and the right primary auditory cortex (BA 42) and right subcentral area (BA 43) related to mothers’ BDI–II scores

Fig. 7
figure 7

Left and superior views of the increased functional connectivity between the right secondary visual cortex [Brodmann area (BA) 18, gray] and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 46) related to maternal depression scores

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to examine the relationship between cumulative maternal depression and the development of neurobiological networks for language processing in the child. We chose to specifically examine functional connections within the visual cortex, a region related to imagination during storytelling, while the child is listening to stories. Per our hypothesis, decreased functional connections between seeds in the visual cortex and auditory processing regions were found in relation to elevated maternal depression levels. Interestingly, these relations were contralateral; i.e., data showed decreased functional connections between right visual regions and left auditory regions and between the left visual regions and right auditory regions. In support of our hypothesis, increased functional connections between the visual cortex and frontal regions were found to be associated with maternal depression; however, this was found only in the right hemisphere. Some possible explanations for this lateralization will be discussed.

Increased maternal depression is related to decreased functional connections between visualization and auditory regions while listening to stories

Imagination is one of the critical components when listening to stories [36]. This finding, together with auditory processing previously observed during stories listening (BA 41 in 5–7 year-old children and bilateral activation in BA 41 in 11-year-old children [33]), highlight the critical role of both visual and auditory regions when listening to stories. Children who are better at comprehending stories reported that they visualize the stories they hear in their imagination, like a movie [37]. It was found that the ability to use visualization while listening to stories also supports future reading abilities in children [33]. However, the ability to visualize is not equal when listening to stories (as was required in the current study) versus reading a book or listening to a story where there is a greater visual stimulation [38]. Therefore, listening to stories requires greater attention abilities and imagination due to a lack of visual stimulation and the focus on the reader’s voice [38]. However, mothers with depression were found to read to and communicate with their children less [39, 40]. Additionally, maternal or paternal elevated depression levels in early childhood (9 and 24 months) are related to decreased reading time to their children Paulson [41]. In addition to the quantity of reading, stories reading “style” differed between depressed and non-depressed mothers, whereas depressed mothers read for a shorter time and asked fewer questions when reading the story [39, 40], though the results of this study were not compared to non-depressed mothers. Despite the lack of studies characterizing specifically the level of interaction during storytelling to their children, focusing on the child (i.e., asking children if their visualization the stories or how comfortable they feel while their mothers tell them stories) we do know that emotional regions are directly intertwined with visual processing [42]. Therefore, we submit that the decreased functional connections between the visual and auditory regions is correlated with increased maternal depression levels because the children of depressed mothers may have fewer mental resources available for visualization of the stories. These children were thought to suffer from emotional difficulties as well as negative affective states [43], which may interfere with allocating attention to visualizing the stories and enjoy the joint reading activity. A future study explicitly examining this point by asking the children if they visualized the stories, or alternatively a prospective study with scans performed at baseline and following intervention to instruct mothers how to read stories to their children dialogically would be useful to further clarify this point.

Maternal depression is associated with greater connectivity between a child’s right visual cortex and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

Our study suggests a positive correlation in functional connectivity between visualization and right frontal regions (right BA 46). Although cognitive control is important in order to focus and process verbal information [44], along with occipital regions engaged in both linguistic processing and stories listening (see also [45] in children with dyslexia), the involvement of the right frontal hemisphere rather than the left frontal hemisphere, is intriguing. The activation in the right hemisphere was previously related to depression symptoms, anxiety and overall stress levels (see [46] for review). The right frontal lobe, as opposed to the left, was related to greater emotional disturbance and a lack of empathy [47]. Children of mothers with maternal depression demonstrated greater EEG electrical signal in the right frontal lobe versus the left and showed less empathy to their environment. Both infants and toddlers of depressed mothers exhibit an overall increased right frontal electrical signal versus the left hemisphere Sohr-Preston [10]. Hence, increased functional connections between the right visual region and the right frontal region may represent, in addition to the load in cognitive control during imagination while listening to stories, less positive emotional processing during the visualization process in children of mothers with elevated depression levels.

Alternations in interhemispheric functional connections in children of depressed mothers

Our study indicates decreased functional connections between right visual and left auditory regions, as well as between left visual and right auditory regions, which raises a question regarding the relationship between maternal depression and the cross-hemispheric information transfer through the corpus callosum. Short corpus callosum was previously related to less responsive maternal behavior [48]. Kok and colleagues reviewed the literature on associations between executive function problems and a shorter corpus callosum in young children. The authors suggested that parenting style, which is related to executive functions (i.e., lack of discipline and lack of sensitivity to the child and the child’s needs), was also related to a shorter corpus callosum. It was also suggested that increased maternal depression levels may affect the development of the corpus callosum, perhaps leading to decreased functional connections between two key brain regions critical for stories listening (visual and auditory) in crossing hemispheres. However, the shorter corpus callosum described by Kok and colleagues may not be directly related to decreased interhemispheric functional activity. Therefore, using visual or auditory tasks activating one of the hemispheres and detecting the activation in the other, as related to maternal depression, may provide more accurate information related to the interhemispheric connections related to maternal depression.

Study limitations

The results of this study may be influenced by several limitations. Our results are based on a relatively small number of participants (N = 21). Cumulative maternal depression levels at 4 years may be related to different brain activation patterns along development. A longitudinal study examining children of mothers with depression versus children of mothers without depression should be explored. To identify neural circuits related to maternal depression in children, an additional study should include a control group of non-depressed mothers. No correlations were found between BDI and child linguistic measures as examined in the current study (WIPSSI vocabulary and CTOPP phonological awareness measures). A future study should examine comprehension level and assess the correlations of those measures with BDI to strengthen the connection between comprehension and BDI. Lastly, although the focus of this study was on the relationship between maternal depression and visualization during stories listening in their children, there are additional abilities and related networks involved in this process (i.e., language and cognitive control [36, 49]). These networks may also be related to maternal depression and should be examined in depth. Last but not least, recent studies demonstrated the association between maternal depression and altered prefrontal functional connectivity in the offspring [50]. Additional studies pointed at the association between the use of antidepressant medications during the prenatal period and changes in offspring brain connectivity [51]. Hence, in the current study design it is impossible excluding the potential prenatal confounds on the current study results. This point should be clarified using longitudinal studies starting from pregnancy.

Conclusions

The current study suggests that cumulative maternal depression levels are related to decreased functional connections between a child’s visualization regions and auditory regions that process verbal information. The reliance on frontal regions, related not only to executive functions but also to negative emotions in children, as demonstrated. These results highlight the need to monitor maternal depression during regular office visits and also even before birth since prenatal maternal depression has been associated with altered connectivity in infants [52]. Prenatal exposure and genetic depression risk may contribute to altered connectivity.

The impact of maternal depression during early childhood on neural circuitry of story listening suggests the importance of dialogic reading between mother and child. Introducing engaging reading methods to depressed mothers might improve child brain development by providing a structured context for optimal verbal stimulation and interactions with their mothers.

Summary

The results of the current study indicate that maternal depression was correlated with decreased functional connectivity between visualization and auditory regions and increased functional connectivity in frontal regions. These findings suggest decreased imagination and increased negative emotions in children of depressed mothers and highlight the need for engaging reading methods, especially for depressed mothers and their children.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable.

References

  1. Kuhl PK. Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition. Neuron. 2010;67(5):713–27.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Steinbeis N, Bernhardt BC, Singer T. Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron. 2012;73(5):1040–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Vannest JJ, et al. Comparison of fMRI data from passive listening and active-response story processing tasks in children. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2009;29(4):971–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK, Plante E. Cognitive modules utilized for narrative comprehension in children: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage. 2006;29(1):254–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Berl MM, Duke ES, Mayo J, Rosenberger LR, Moore EN, VanMeter J, Gaillard WD. Functional anatomy of listening and reading comprehension during development. Brain Lang. 2010;114(2):115–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2010.06.002.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Hutton JS, et al. Home reading environment and brain activation in preschool children listening to stories. Pediatrics. 2015;136(3):466–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. De Koning BB, van der Schoot M. Becoming part of the story! Refueling the interest in visualization strategies for reading comprehension. Educ Psychol Rev. 2013;25:261–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Kosslyn SM, Pascual-Leone A, Felician O, Camposano S, Keenan JP, Thompson WL, Ganis G, Sukel KE, Alpert NM. The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. Science. 1999;284:167–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Horowitz-Kraus T, Hutton J, Phelan K, Holland SK. Maternal reading fluency is positively associated with greater functional connectivity between the child’s future reading network and regions related to executive functions and language processing in preschool-age children. Brain Cogn. 2018;121:17–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Sohr-Preston SL, Scaramella LV. Implications of timing of maternal depressive symptoms for early cognitive and language development. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2006;9(1):65–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Ammerman RT, et al. Changes in depressive symptoms in first time mothers in home visitation. Child Abuse Negl. 2009;33(3):127–38.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Ammerman RT, et al. Impact of depression and childhood trauma in mothers receiving home visitation. J Child Fam Stud. 2012;21:612–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Breznitz Z. Verbal indicators of depression. J Gen Psychol. 2001;119:351–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Field T. Infants of depressedmothers. Infant Behav Dev. 1995;18:1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Martinez A, Malphurs J, Field T, Pickens J, Yando R, Bendell D, et al. Depressed mothers’ and their infants’ interactions with nondepressed partners. Infant Ment Health J. 1996;17:74–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Bettes B. Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features. Child Dev. 1988;59:1089–96.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Nichols KE, Fox N, Mundy P. Joint attention, self-recognition, and neurocognitive function in toddlers. Infancy. 2005;7(1):35–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Hutton JS, Dudley J, Horowitz-Kraus T, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Differences in functional brain network connectivity during stories presented in audio, illustrated, and animated format in preschool-age children. Brain Imaging Behavr. 2018;14:130–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Gazzaley A, Rissman J, Cooney J, Rutman A, Seibert T, Clapp W, D’Esposito M. functional interactions between prefrontal and visual association cortex contribute to top-down modulation of visual processing. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:i125–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Rajagopal A, et al. Success rates for functional MR imaging in children. Am J Neuroradiol. 2014;35:2319–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Yuan W, Altaye M, Ret J, Schmithorst V, Byars AW, Plante E, Holland SK. Quantification of head motion in children during various fMRI language tasks. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(5):1481–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Phelan KJ, et al. The Cincinnati Home Injury Prevention (CHIP) and literacy promotion trial: 24-month follow-up. San Francisco: Pediatric Academic Societies; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Wechsler D. The Wechsler adult intelligence scale (WAIS)—IV. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA. Comprehensive test of phonological processing (CTOPP). Austin: Pro-Ed; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA, Wagner RK. TOWRE: test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory-II, No. 2, vol. 78. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1996. p. 490–8.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Wang Y, Dix T. Mothers’ depressive symptoms in infancy and children’s adjustment in grade school: the role of children’s sustained attention and executive function. Dev Psychol. 2017;53(9):1666.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Byars AW, et al. Practical aspects of conducting large-scale functional magnetic resonance imaging studies in children. J Child Neurol. 2002;17(12):885–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Vannest J, et al. Factors determining success of awake and asleep magnetic resonance imaging scans in nonsedated children. Neuropediatrics. 2014;45(6):370–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Holland SK, Vannest J, Mecoli M, Jacola LM, Tillema JM, Karunanayaka PR, Schmithorst VJ, Yuan W, Plante E, Byars AW. Functional MRI of language lateralization during development in children. Int J Audiol. 2007;46(9):533–51.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Smith S, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23(S1):208–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Szaflarski JP, et al. A longitudinal functional magnetic resonance imaging study of language development in children 5 to 11 years old. Ann Neurol. 2006;59(5):796–807.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Horowitz-Kraus T, Vannest JJ, Holland SK. Overlapping neural circuitry for narrative comprehension and proficient reading in children and adolescents. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(13):2651–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connect. 2012;2(3):125–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Baria AT, et al. Anatomical and functional assemblies of brain BOLD oscillations. J Neurosci. 2011;31(21):7910–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Karunanayaka PR, et al. Age-related connectivity changes in fMRI data from children listening to stories. Neuroimage. 2007;34(1):349–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. McTigue EM. Teaching young readers imagery in storytelling: what color is the monkey? Read Teach. 2010;64:53–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Isbell B, Sobol J, Lindauer J, Lowrance A. The effects of storytelling and story reading on the oral language complexity and story comprehension of young children. Early Child Educ J. 2004;32(3):157–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Reissland N, Shepherd J, Herrera E. The pitch of maternal voice: a comparison of mothers suffering from depressed mood and non-depressed mothers reading books to their infants. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:255–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Bigatti SM, Cronan TA, Anaya A. The effects of maternal depression on the efficacy of a literacy intervention program. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2001;32:147–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Paulson JF, Keefe HA, Leiferman JA. Early parental depression and child language development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(3):254–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Pessoa L. On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(2):148.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Tronick E, Reck C. Infants of depressed mothers. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2009;17:147–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Horowitz-Kraus T, Farah R, DiFrancesco M, Vannest J. The involvement of speed-of-processing in story listening in preschool children: a functional and structural connectivity study. Neuropediatrics. 2016;48:19–29.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Horowitz-Kraus T, Buck C, Dorrmann D. Altered neural circuits accompany lower performance during narrative comprehension in children with reading difficulties: an fMRI study. Ann Dyslexia. 2016;66:301–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Hecht D. Depression and the hyperactive right-hemisphere. Neurosci Res. 2010;68:77–87.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Jones NA, Field T, Davalos M, Gables C. Right frontal EEG asymmetry and lack of empathy in preschool children of depressed mothers. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2000;30(3):189–204.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Kok R, Lucassen N, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH, Ghassabian A, Roza SJ, Govaert P, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Tiemeier H. Parenting, corpus callosum, and executive function in preschool children. Child Neuropsychol. 2014;20(5):583–606.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Farah R, Horowitz-Kraus T. Increased functional connectivity within and between cognitive-control networks from early infancy to 9 years during stories listening: a developmental task-based functional MRI study. Brain Connect. 2019;9:385–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Posner J, Cha J, Roy A, Peterson BS, Bansal R, Gustafsson HC, Raffanello E, Gingrich J, Monk C. Alterations in amygdala–prefrontal circuits in infants exposed to prenatal maternal depression. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6:e935.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Lugo-Candelas C, Cha J, Hong S, Bastidas V, Weissman M, Fifer WP, Myers M, Talati A, Bansal R, Peterson BS, Monk C, Gingrich JA, Posner J. Associations between brain structure and connectivity in infants and exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172(6):525–33.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Qiu A, Anh TT, Li Y, Chen H, Rifkin-Graboi A, Broekman BF, Kwek K, Saw S, Chong Y, Gluckman P, Fortier M, Meaney M. Prenatal maternal depression alters amygdala functional connectivity in 6-month-old infants. Transl Psychiatry. 2015;5:e508.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors acknowledge the participation and support of Every Child Succeeds and its funders, the United Way of Greater Cincinnati, Kentucky H.A.N.D.S., and Ohio Help Me Grow. We would also like to acknowledge Stacey Liddy, MS, in the CCHMC Department of Pediatric Emergency Medicine, and the HOME Study research assistants for aiding in data collection and recruitment. The authors thank J. Denise Wetzel for review and editing of the manuscript.

Funding

This study in part was supported by Grant Number R01HD066115 from the National Institute of Child Health and Development to Dr. Phelan and by Interagency Agreement from the U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

THK designed and supervised all aspects of the study including the fMRI protocol, fMRI data processing and analysis and drafted the manuscript and subsequent revisions. RF conducted all the fMRI data analysis and drafted the manuscript, created all tables and figures, and assisted with manuscript preparation and revision. PG assisted in data analysis and revised the manuscript. JH, RA, SKH, KJP and JD provided guidance on study design and analysis, assisted with the fMRI acquisition protocol, and reviewed and revised the manuscript. SKH also provided guidance on study design and analysis, helped develop and oversaw the MRI acquisition protocol, and reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

The authors would like to thank Stacey Liddy-Hicks and Stacey Woeste, the research coordinators in this study, for working with the participants and families, and for assisting in data collection and scoring. The authors would also like to thank the MRI technologists: Lacey Haas, Matt Lanier, Brynne Williams and Kaley Bridgewater for working with the children in the MR environment.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Institutional and/or National Research Committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Consent for publication

The corresponding author, on behalf all authors, allows the publication of this Manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Farah, R., Greenwood, P., Dudley, J. et al. Maternal depression is associated with altered functional connectivity between neural circuits related to visual, auditory, and cognitive processing during stories listening in preschoolers. Behav Brain Funct 16, 5 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-020-00167-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-020-00167-5

Keywords