Skip to main content

Transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation affect explicit but not implicit emotion regulation: a meta-analysis

Abstract

Emotion regulation (ER) refers to the process through which people influence the occurrence, experience, and expression of emotions. It can be established in an explicit (voluntary) or implicit (automatic) way, both of which are essential for mental and physical well-being. Recent evidence has highlighted the potential of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) targeting the prefrontal cortex (PFC) to modulate ER. However, previous studies have only evaluated the effects of TMS and tDCS on explicit ER, leaving implicit ER relatively unexplored. In this review and meta-analysis, we systematically evaluated the effects of TMS and tDCS over the PFC on the two forms of ER, using both subjective and physiological response as outcome indicators. Twenty-seven studies were included in our study. Both subjective (Hedges’ g = − 0.20) and physiological (Hedges’ g = − 0.65) results indicated a significant effect of TMS and tDCS targeting PFC on down-regulation of explicit ER, but not implicit ER (Hedges’ g = − 0.04). Moreover, moderation analysis indicated that the effect of TMS and tDCS on the down-regulating of subjective experience was moderated by several factors, including stimulation method, target area, target hemisphere, and stimulation timing. Specifically, our results showed that applying TMS or targeting the right PFC, particularly the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, or using offline TMS and tDCS produced a larger stimulation effect on ER. In summary, these findings suggest that TMS and tDCS has a positive effect on explicit, but not implicit ER. The distinct TMS and tDCS effect on the two forms of ER help deepen our understanding of TMS and tDCS use and provide valuable insights for the development of tailored TMS and tDCS protocols for explicit and implicit regulation.

Introduction

Emotion regulation (ER) involves individuals modifying their emotional responses to behave appropriately when encountering various social situations, which is essential for maintaining both physical and mental health [1,2,3]. The cognitive framework of ER suggests that this process occurs either voluntarily (explicit ER) or automatically (implicit ER) [4, 5]. Explicit ER runs with a conscious effort to change emotional responses and requires conscious monitoring, while implicit ER begins automatically and involves the change of emotional responses without monitoring [6]. To assess ER processes/outcomes, studies usually use participants’ subjective experiences (e.g., emotional intensity and valence rating) and physiological indexes (e.g., skin conductance response and pupil dilation) [7,8,9,10]. However, alterations in these measures do not consistently mirror each other across distinct ER tasks. Suppression, for example, reduces skin conductance response, leaving emotional intensity unaffected [11, 12], whereas cognitive reappraisal diminishes negative emotional experiences without affecting heart rates [13].

Neuroimaging studies have suggested that explicit and implicit ER both critically involve the prefrontal cortex (PFC) but with specific different PFC subregions. Explicit ER largely recruits the lateral PFC, namely the dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) and ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC) [14, 15]. In contrast, implicit ER engages more with the medial PFC (MPFC), especially ventral MPFC (VMPFC) [16,17,18]. ER pursues two different regulation goals, down-regulation (diminishing emotion) and up-regulation (intensifying emotion) [19]. These two goals also are associated with distinct PFC regions: down-regulation is associated more with the right PFC activity, while up-regulation is related more with the left PFC activity [20, 21].

Nevertheless, conclusions deriving from neuroimaging techniques are largely correlational and the causal inference between PFC functioning and ER could not be derived. Non-invasive brain stimulation could temporally modify brain excitability without harm [22], which is a promising tool to investigate such causal relationships. Non-invasive brain stimulation includes transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) techniques. tES applies various current waveforms, including transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS), transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS), and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS), to the scalp, modulating neuronal states [23]. Among these, tDCS is the most commonly used protocol, delivering low-intensity electrical current (typically 1–2 mA) to the superficial brain regions, thereby modifying cortical excitability [24, 25]. Anodal tDCS enhances cortical activity, while cathodal tDCS exerts the opposite effect [26, 27]. In contrast, TMS applies brief, high-intensity magnetic pulses to the scalp, inducing electric fields that alter neural activity [28]. TMS can be administered as single-pulse TMS (spTMS) or repetitive TMS (rTMS). The effects of rTMS depend on its frequency: low-frequency rTMS (< 1 Hz) or intermittent theta burst stimulation (iTBS) exert inhibitory effects, while high-frequency rTMS (> 5 Hz) or theta burst stimulation (cTBS) can induce excitatory effects [29, 30]. tDCS and TMS are two commonly used brain stimulation methods. Research has demonstrated the efficacy of TMS and tDCS targeting the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in modulating emotion and emotion perception [31,32,33,34]. Furthermore, emerging evidence suggests that rTMS and anodal tDCS can enhance PFC activity during emotion regulation, potentially improving emotional regulation abilities [35,36,37,38,39]. However, since individual TMS and tDCS studies on ER varies in stimulation protocols, forms of ER, and measurement methods, evidence is not consistent in all studies and therefore the causal relationship remains inconclusive. Quantifying the TMS and tDCS effects on ER is expected to (1) refine the scope of TMS and tDCS application to maximize its ER-modulating effect, thereby providing an efficient way for people to improve their emotional health and general well-being, and (2) benefit the treatment of ER deficits in psychiatric disorders such as anxiety [40] and depression [41].

TMS and tDCS modulatory effects on ER have been partially summarized in two meta-analysis studies [42, 43]. Specifically, Smith and colleagues demonstrated the efficacy of tDCS in decreasing stress-related emotional reactivity, which may be attributed to the effect of anodal tDCS on ER [42]. This finding indirectly suggests that TMS and tDCS may influence ER process to reduce negative emotional responses. While Zhang and colleagues provided direct evidence that TMS and tDCS reduced negative emotions during down-regulation [43]. However, these studies did not include studies aimed to up-regulate emotion; furthermore, explicit and implicit ER were not differentiated in prior work. Considering that explicit (including both up- and down-regulation) and implicit ER have been demonstrated to be associated with distinct neural representations in PFC [5, 6], it is expected that the effects of TMS and tDCS targeting PFC on explicit and implicit ER may differ. Therefore, a systematic review of the literature on explicit and implicit ER is needed for a comprehensive understanding of the effect of TMS and tDCS on ER. Distinguishing and comparing the TMS and tDCS effects on different forms of ER may also help develop individualized TMS and tDCS protocols targeting various ER deficits.

The current meta-analysis aimed to provide a comprehensive overview of the TMS and tDCS effects on ER, with an assumption that TMS and tDCS differentially modulates the explicit and implicit ER. Considering the potential inconsistency between subjective and physiological on ER, it is necessary to evaluate the effect of TMS and tDCS on ER by using various types of measurements. Thus, in addition to the self-reported emotional feelings [42, 43], we also included physiological responses such as skin conductance response and pupil dilation because they provided objective indices for the effect of TMS and tDCS on ER. Given the high heterogeneity observed in previous meta-analyses [43], we further investigated whether stimulation method and stimulation parameter (e.g., targeted area/hemisphere, stimulation timing, and stimulation duration) moderate the effect of TMS and tDCS on ER. In addition, studies have shown that the cognitive resources recruited during ER differ between general affective pictures and specific affective stimuli during ER. The former, induced by the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) [44], are complex in emotional content and require more cognitive resources, whereas the latter, induced by specific affective stimuli (e.g., pain and memory), exhibit less complex and heterogeneous content, which need relatively fewer cognitive resources [21]. Therefore, it can be speculated that stimuli type may affect the effect of TMS and tDCS on ER.

Methods

Literature search

Following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [45], a literature search was conducted by two trained investigators (Xiufu Qiu & Zhenhong He) using the PubMed, Web of Science, and Scopus electronic databases to obtain studies on TMS and tDCS and ER from the earliest publication dates available to March 2023. The combination of keywords “TMS or tDCS or tACS or tRNS” and “ER” was utilized in the search, which was limited to human studies and English-language publications. The detailed search terms can be found in Part 1 of the Additional file 1. Following Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions [46], reference lists from similar reviews and meta-analyses were also screened for relevant studies [42, 43, 47,48,49,50,51]. This study was pre-registered on the Open Science Forum platform (https://osf.io/87t6s).

Eligibility criteria

Studies that met the following criteria were included in the meta-analysis: (1) Studies were published in English journals. (2) Participants were healthy human adults aged 18–60 years old. (3) TMS and tDCS was administered over PFC before or during the ER task. The examples of excitatory TMS and tDCS included high-frequency rTMS, iTBS, anodal tDCS, while the examples of inhibitory TMS and tDCS were low-frequency rTMS, cTBS, and cathodal tDCS [23, 52]. (4) The TMS and tDCS protocol included a sham or control condition. For the sham condition of TMS, stimulation was administered through either a sham coil, a tilted coil, or vertex stimulation [53]. For the sham condition of tDCS, a short (usually 30–60 s) application of current was applied at the beginning of tasks and gradually switched off [54]. (5) Studies used explicit ER (i.e., reappraisal, distraction, suppression, distancing, and placebo) or implicit ER tasks (i.e., extinction, reinforcer revaluation, emotional Go/No-Go, emotional Stroop, affect labeling, automatic goal pursuit, and reversal learning) [5]. (6) The effect of TMS and tDCS on ER was measured by the self-reported scores and/or physiological responses, including valence, arousal, and intensity, skin conductance response, fear-potentiated startle, pupil dilation, and facial electromyography.

Data extraction

Two investigators (Xiufu Qiu & Zhenhong He) independently screened the title, abstract, and full text of the studies. They then extracted all relevant data from the final included articles. Any disagreement was settled by a panel discussion with a third investigator (Dandan Zhang). Specifically, there were 16 disagreements out of 270, which represents a relatively small proportion. Each case was thoroughly reviewed by the three-person group until a consensus was reached. For each article, the following data were extracted: author’s name, publication years, study design, sample size, the stimulation protocol (stimulation method, target area, stimulation parameters, control condition, stimulation timing), affective evoking material (i.e., general affective pictures or specific affective stimuli), task types (e.g., ER task or fear extinction), ER goals (down-regulation or up-regulation), the outcome measures of ER and the results of TMS and tDCS on ER (including the results of subjective experience or physiological response).

For the outcome measures of ER, we extracted the mean (M), standard deviation (SD), and sample size (N) in each condition or group (i.e., active and sham) for further quantitative analyses. The outcome measures were adjusted if necessary. First, in most ER studies, a higher self-reported score indicates more negative emotion. If the study used a reversed scale (i.e., a higher score indicates more positive emotion), the group mean values were normalized to get in line with the typical scale. The equations for the normalization procedure are as follows:

$$ {\text{If}}\,X_{{{\text{med}}}} = \, 0,{\text{ then}}\,X_{{{\text{new}}}} = \, - X_{{{\text{original}}}} $$
$$ {\text{If}}\,X_{{{\text{med}}}} \ne \, 0,{\text{then}}\,X_{{{\text{new}}}} = X_{{{\text{max}}}} - X_{{{\text{original}}}} + {1} $$

where Xmed denotes the median score of the scale, Xnew denotes the normalized mean score, Xoriginal denotes the original mean score, and Xmax denotes the maximum scale score used in the study. Second, if the study provided the standard error (SE) instead of SD, SE was converted to SD through the formulas SD = SE ×\(\sqrt{N}\) [46]. Third, if relevant data were unavailable, we reached out to the corresponding authors. If the data were unable to provide, the data displayed in the figures were identified and extracted by WebPlotDigitizer [55, 56]. We excluded literature only when we cannot obtain data through the above methods.

Data analysis

All quantitative analyses were performed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis V3 (CMA, Bio-Englewood, New Jersey, US) [57]. We separately conducted meta-analyses for TMS and tDCS excitability (excitatory and inhibitory TMS and tDCS) and ER measurement (subjective experience and physiological response). For both explicit and implicit ER, we entered the M, SD, and N of the active and sham group into the CMA. Considering that explicit ER involves bidirectional goals (implicit ER typically involves only unidirectional goals, i.e., down-regulation) [20], meta-analyses of down-regulation and up-regulation were also performed separately. In addition, we also performed the above calculates in the no-regulation condition (looking passively at the affective pictures or electric shocks always paired with conditioned stimuli) to ensure the effects of TMS and tDCS were specific to ER processing, rather than general cognitive alteration.

A random-effects model was performed for each meta-analysis due to the methodological diversity among included studies. For each outcome measure, effect size (Hedges’ g) was calculated to assess the effect of TMS and tDCS on ER, which can correct the small sample bias [58]. The values of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 indicate small, medium, and large effects [59]. Negative values indicate decreased, while positive values indicate increased negative emotional response in active condition compared to sham condition. In general, each study only generated one effect size. If a study reported multiple outcomes from the same participant group, such as different outcome measurements (e.g., valence and arousal) or multiple time points (e.g., early stage and late stage), it may result in multiple effect sizes. However, these multiple effect sizes cannot be independently treated as it would lead to incorrect estimates of the variance for the summary effect size [60]. Therefore, we combined these multiple effect sizes by CMA to obtain an average effect size for each study. For heterogeneity between studies, we used Cochran’s Q to identify the presence of heterogeneity and accordingly the I2 was used to measure the magnitude of the heterogeneity, with the values of 25%, 50%, and 75% indicating a small, medium, and large degree of heterogeneity [59]. Funnel plot and Egger’s test were used to evaluate the publication bias if the meta-analysis contains at least 10 different studies [46, 61, 62]. To check the robustness of the results, we conducted a sensitivity analysis using the one-study-removed method with CMA. For all statistical analyses, a p-value < 0.05 (two-tailed) was considered significant.

Finally, moderation analysis was used to explore whether stimulation method (i.e. tDCS, TMS) and stimulation parameters including target hemisphere (left and right PFC), target area (regions for explicit ER: lDLPFC, rDLPFC, lVLPFC, and rVLPFC; regions for implicit ER: lVMPFC, rVMPFC, lDLPFC, rDLPFC, lVLPFC, and rVLPFC), stimulation timing (online and offline), stimulation duration (< 20 min, 20 min, and > 20 min), and stimuli types (general affective pictures and specific affective stimuli) influenced the effect of TMS and tDCS on ER. This analysis was only conducted when sufficient data were available (at least 10 studies) [46].

Risk of bias assessment

Two investigators (Xiufu Qiu & Xueying Cao) independently assessed the risk of bias in each study using the revised Cochrane risk of bias tool (ROB V.2.0) [63]. The following domains were assessed: randomization process, deviations from intended interventions, missing outcome data, measurement of the outcome, and selection of the reported result. The risk of bias for each domain was graded as either low, high, or unclear and then summarized into an overall judgment. A study was regarded as low risk of bias only when all domains were graded as low risk of bias. A study was regarded as unclear risk of bias if one domain was graded as unclear risk of bias and no other domains were graded as high risk of bias. A study was considered as high risk of bias if at least one domain was graded as high risk. Discrepancies between the two investigators were settled by consensus or by a panel discussion with a third investigator.

Results

Included literature and study characteristics

The systematic search yielded 7522 studies from the database and 4 studies from the reference lists of articles. After removing duplicates, the titles and abstracts of 5505 studies were screened for eligibility. Of these, 76 studies underwent full-text evaluation, and 27 studies that fulfilled the eligibility criteria were included in our review. The literature selection process is visualized in an adapted PRISMA flow diagram (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram. PRISMA = Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses

Among the 27 studies in our meta-analyses, 2 studies applied both excitatory and inhibitory TMS and tDCS, 21 studies applied excitatory TMS and tDCS, and 4 studies applied inhibitory TMS and tDCS. Therefore, a total of 23 studies were included in the excitatory TMS and tDCS result and 6 studies were included in the inhibitory TMS and tDCS result. Here, we only reported the excitatory TMS and tDCS results in the main text. For the inhibitory TMS and tDCS results, please see the Part 2 in Additional file 1. Among excitatory TMS and tDCS studies, there were 19 explicit and 4 implicit ER studies and included high-frequency rTMS, iTBS, excitatory spTMS, and anodal tDCS. The targeting brain regions included the DLPFC, VLPFC, and VMPFC. For detailed information on study characteristics, see Tables 1, 2.

Table 1 Characteristics of excitatory TMS of explicit and implicit ER studies
Table 2 Characteristics of anodal tDCS of explicit and implicit ER studies

Risk of bias

A summary of the risk of bias assessment of all included studies is illustrated in Fig. 2. Overall, 8 studies (34.8%) were considered as low risk of bias and 11 studies (47.8%) were assessed as unclear risk of bias mainly due to lack of random sequence generation and allocation concealment (43.5%), while 4 studies (17.4%) showed a high risk of bias because of missing outcome data (13%).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Risk of bias summary of all included studies (n = 23). A Methodological quality assessment of each study at 5 domains was illustrated. B Risk of bias graph

The effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on explicit ER

Subjective experience of down-regulation

The 19 excitatory TMS and tDCS studies reported 24 outcomes on subjective experience of down-regulation and included 14 anodal tDCS studies, 1 spTMS study, and 4 high-frequency rTMS studies. They included 1161 participants, of which 44 underwent both active and sham stimulation, 656 underwent active stimulation and 515 underwent sham stimulation.

A total of 14 studies assessed the effect of anodal tDCS on down-regulation. Four studies targeting the rDLPFC [36, 64] or rVLPFC [38, 41] found a significant anodal tDCS-induced decrease in negative emotional reactivity during down-regulation. The other eight studies primarily stimulated the lDLPFC and didn’t find such an effect [65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72]. Two studies targeting the lDLPFC [73] or lVLPFC [74] found an increase in experienced disgust or arousal after watching negative pictures.

A total of 5 studies assessed the effect of TMS on down-regulation. Two high-frequency rTMS studies targeting the rVLPFC or rDLPFC found a decrease in perceived negative emotion during social pain image presentation [37, 39]. One excitatory spTMS study targeting the lVLPFC also found decreased emotional valence during down-regulating negative pictures [35]. In contrast, two high-frequency rTMS studies observed no effect during down-regulating negative social feedback or negative image after stimulating the VLPFC or DLPFC [75, 76].

The full random-effects model showed a significant excitatory stimulation effect on subjective experience of down-regulation (Hedges’ g = − 0.20; Z-value = − 1.97; 95% CI = [− 0.39, 0.00]; p = 0.049; Fig. 3), which indicates that excitatory stimulation decreased the negative emotional experience during down-regulation compared to the sham condition. Sensitivity analysis showed that the result was robust (see in the Additional file 1: Fig S5). Moderate heterogeneity was observed (Q = 68.61, p < 0.001; I2 = 66.47%). Publication bias was not observed through the visual inspection of the funnel plot (Fig. 4) or Egger’s test (t = 1.20; p = 0.284). The moderation analysis showed that the effect of excitatory stimulation was significantly moderated by the stimulation method (Q = 4.02, p = 0.045), target hemisphere (Q = 9.17, p = 0.002), target area (Q = 22.26, p = 0.000), and stimulation timing (Q = 9.95, p = 0.019). Further analysis of these moderating variables is as follows: for stimulation method, effect sizes of TMS studies were significantly larger than anodal tDCS studies (TMS: g = − 0.43, 95% CI [− 0.62, − 0.23]; p = 0.000; anodal tDCS: g = − 0.10, 95% CI = [− 0.36, 0.17]; p = 0.473). For target hemisphere, effect sizes of right PFC studies were significantly larger than left PFC studies (right PFC: g = − 0.44, 95% CI [− 0.67, − 0.20]; p = 0.000; left PFC: g = 0.11, 95% CI [− 0.15, 0.37]; p = 0.405). In terms of target area, effect sizes of rVLPFC studies were significantly larger than lDLPFC, lVLPFC, and rDLPFC studies (rVLPFC: g = 0.075, 95% CI [− 0.70, − 0.30]; p = 0.000; lDLPFC: g = 0.21, 95% CI [− 0.02, 0.43]; p = 0.473; lVLPFC: g = -0.02, 95% CI [− 0.63, 0.60]; p = 0.961; rDLPFC: g = -0.42, 95% CI [− 0.85, 0.02]; p = 0.059). For stimulation timing, effect sizes of offline studies were significantly larger than online studies (offline: g = − 0.52, 95% CI [− 0.75, − 0.28]; p = 0.000; online: g = − 0.05, 95% CI [− 0.29, 0.18]; p = 0.405). We also compared brain subregions within left and right PFC separately. Within the right PFC,no significant difference was found between the rDLPFC and rVLPFC (Q = 0.12, p = 0.734). Similarly, within the left PFC, no significant difference was found between the lDLPFC and rVLPFC (Q = 0.44, p = 0.506). No other significant moderators were found (p > 0.05). Details of the moderation analysis were shown in Table 3.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on the subjective experience of down-regulation. Combined: Studies with multiple outcomes (e.g., valence and arousal) within a study were combined into an averaged data with CMA, which can prevent an improper estimate of the precision of the summary effect

Fig. 4
figure 4

Funnel plot for the excitatory TMS and tDCS effect on the subjective experience of down-regulation, which shows no publication bias; the Egger’s test is non-significant (p = 0.265)

Table 3 Moderation analysis results for the subjective experience of down-regulation

Physiological response of down-regulation

We identified 3 anodal tDCS studies that reported 3 outcomes of the physiological response of down-regulation. These studies included 180 participants, of which 92 underwent active stimulation and 88 underwent sham stimulation. These studies targeting the rDLPFC [36] or rVLPFC [38, 41] found a significant decrease in skin conductance response or pupil dilation to negative stimulation after anodal tDCS.

Excitatory stimulation effect on physiological response of down-regulation was significant (Hedges’ g = -0.65, Z-value = − 4.26, 95% CI [− 0.94, − 0.35], p < 0.001; Fig. 5). Sensitivity analysis showed that the result was robust (see in the Additional file 1: Fig S6). Low heterogeneity was observed (Q = 1.33; p = 0.514; I2 = 0.00%). Due to the small sample size, moderation analysis could not be performed.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on the physiological response of down-regulation. Combined: Studies with multiple physiological outcomes (e.g. individual negative and social negative image) within a study were combined into an averaged data with CMA, which can prevent an improper estimate of the precision of the summary effect; SCR  skin conductance response, PD pupil dilation

Subjective experience of up-regulation

We identified 5 anodal tDCS studies that reported 8 outcomes on the subjective experience of up-regulation. These studies included 263 participants, of which 190 underwent active stimulation and 133 underwent sham stimulation. Only one study targeting rDLPFC found an increase in subjective emotional arousal following anodal tDCS [36]. Three studies found no effect [69, 70, 74]. The other study found that anodal tDCS inhibited up-regulation during the nocebo hyperalgesia task [72].

Excitatory stimulation effect on subjective experience of up-regulation was not significant (Hedges’ g = 0.38, Z-value = 1.39, 95% CI [− 0.15, 0.92], p = 0.165; Fig. 6). Sensitivity analysis showed that the result was robust (see in the Additional file 1: Fig S7). High heterogeneity was observed (Q = 46.71; p < 0.001; I2 = 85.01%). Due to the small sample size, moderation analysis could not be performed.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on the subjective experience of upregulation. Combined: Studies with multiple outcomes (e.g. valence and arousal) within a study were combined into an averaged data with CMA, which can prevent an improper estimate of the precision of the summary effect

Physiological response of up-regulation

We identified one study examining the effect of excitatory stimulation on the physiological response of up-regulation [36], which found an increase in SCR.

The effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on implicit ER

Subjective experience

We identified one study examining the effect of excitatory stimulation on the subjective experience of implicit ER [77], which found no effect.

Physiological response

We identified 4 studies examining the effect of excitatory stimulation on physiological arousal of implicit ER. These studies included 271 participants, of which 44 underwent both active and sham stimulation, 133 underwent active stimulation and 117 underwent sham stimulation. One tDCS study targeting VMPFC found a decrease in the SCR to an aversive stimulus [78]. However, one tDCS study and two high-frequency rTMS studies found no effect [77, 79, 80].

The full random effects model showed no excitatory stimulation effect (Hedges’ g = − 0.04, Z-value = − 0.24, 95% CI [− 0.40, 0.30], p = 0.810; Fig. 7). Sensitivity analysis showed that the result is robust (see in the Additional file 1: Fig S8). Moderate heterogeneity was observed (Q = 9.77, p = 0.044; I2 = 59.06%). Due to the small sample size, moderation analysis could not be performed.

Fig. 7
figure 7

Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on the physiological response of implicit ER. Combined: Studies with multiple outcomes (e.g. SCR and FPS) within a study were combined into averaged data with CMA, which can prevent an improper estimate of the precision of the summary effect; SCR skin conductance response, FPS fear-potentiated startle

The effect of TMS and tDCS on no-regulation condition

There was no significant TMS and tDCS effect on the self-reported and physiological results (see part 3 in Additional file 1). However, we found a decrease on the physiological response in the control condition of down-regulation after excitatory TMS and tDCS (Hedges’ g = − 0.63, Z-value = − 0.93, 95% CI [− 0.93, − 0.34], p < 0.001). As a result, we performed a meta-analysis of the down-regulation advantage (the differential rating between no-regulation and down-regulation condition) to further interpret the effect of TMS and tDCS on physiological response of down-regulation (see also [41]). The results showed a significant TMS and tDCS effect on physiological response of down-regulation advantage (Hedges’ g = 0.40, Z-value = 2.70, 95% CI [0.11, 0.49], p = 0.007; Fig. 8), which is consistent with the result of physiological response of down-regulation.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on the physiological response of down-regulation advantage. Combined: Studies with multiple outcomes (e.g. individual negative and social negative image) within a study were combined into an averaged data with CMA, which can prevent an improper estimate of the precision of the summary effect; SCR skin conductance response, PD  pupil dilation

Comparative analysis of types of ER, types of measurement, and ER goals

First, moderation analysis was performed on types of ER as a moderator variable during explicit ER (including only down-regulation). A significant difference was found when comparing explicit and implicit ER in physiological response (Q = 5.33, p = 0.021): there was a positive effect of TMS and tDCS on explicit ER (k = 3, g = − 0.65, 95% CI [− 0.94, − 0.39], p < 0.001) compared with implicit ER (k = 4, g = − 0.03, 95% CI [− 0.46, 0.39], p = 0.881). Due to the small sample size for implicit ER (k = 1), the comparison using subjective experience could not be performed.

Moderation analysis was performed on types of ER as a moderator variable during explicit ER (including both down- and up-regulation). A significant difference was found when comparing explicit and implicit ER in physiological response (Q = 5.17, p = 0.023): there was a positive effect of TMS and tDCS on explicit ER (k = 3, g = − 0.64, 95% CI [− 0.93, − 0.34], p < 0.001) compared with implicit ER (k = 4, g = − 0.03, 95% CI [− 0.47, 0.40], p = 0.880). Due to the small sample size for implicit ER (k = 1), the comparison using subjective experience could not be performed.

Second, moderation analysis was performed on ER goals as a moderator variable during explicit ER (because implicit ER does not have an ER goal). There was no significant difference when comparing down- (k = 24, g = − 0.20, 95% CI [− 0.39, 0.00], p = 0.048) and up-regulation goals (k = 8, g = 0.38, 95% CI [− 0.16, 0.92], p = 0.165) in subjective experience (Q = 0.40, P = 0.528). Due to the small sample size for up-regulation (k = 1), the comparison using physiological response could not be performed.

Third, moderation analysis was performed on Types of measurement as a moderator variable explicit ER (including only down-regulation). We observed a significant difference when comparing subjective and physiological responses (Q = 6.14, p = 0.013): TMS and tDCS can effectively modulate physiological response (k = 3, g = − 0.65, 95% CI [− 0.94, − 0.35]; p < 0.001) of down-regulation compared with subjective experience (k = 19, g = − 0.17; 95% CI [− 0.42, 0.08]; p = 0.189).

Moderation analysis was performed on Types of measurement as a moderator variable during explicit ER (including both down- and up-regulation). We also observed a significant difference when comparing subjective and physiological responses (Q = 4.78, p = 0.030): TMS and tDCS can effectively modulate physiological response (k = 3, g = − 0.64, 95% CI [− 0.93, − 0.34]; p < 0.001) of down-regulation compared with subjective experience (k = 19, g = − 0.21; 95% CI [− 0.46, 0.04]; p = 0.106).

Discussion

The meta-analysis investigated the potential effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on ER. Both subjective experience and physiological indexes indicated a significant TMS and tDCS effect on explicit ER (down-regulation), but not implicit ER. In addition, the identified TMS and tDCS effect on down-regulation during explicit ER were moderated by factors including stimulation method, target area/hemisphere, and stimulation timing.

Effects of TMS and tDCS on explicit and implicit ER

For down-regulation of explicit ER, meta-analysis indicated that TMS and tDCS had a positive effect on subjective experience outcomes (Hedges’ g = − 0.20). Such an effect was also identified in a previous meta-analysis, as evidenced by a prominent TMS and tDCS -evoked decrease in self-reported negative emotion [43]. Notably, we also observed a similar and stronger TMS and tDCS effect on physiological outcomes (Hedges’ g = − 0.65), which further validates the effectiveness of TMS and tDCS on down-regulation in a more objective way. However, it is important to note that a direct comparison between subjective and physiological outcomes revealed a significant difference (p = 0.013), indicating that TMS and tDCS can effectively modulate physiological outcomes of down-regulation but may have limited impact on subjective experience outcomes. One possible explanation for this discrepancy is that the result of subjective experience showed a high heterogeneity in methodology and was modulated by factors stimulation method, target area, target hemisphere, and stimulation timing (see moderation analysis result). Although the results of this study suggest that subjective emotional experiences and physiological responses are incongruent, it is important to note, as described in the Introduction, that these two indicators do not represent identical meanings. Readers should be mindful of this when interpreting the findings. For up-regulation studies, no significant TMS and tDCS effect was found. Evidence demonstrated that up-regulation was associated with more left-lateralized PFC activity, while down-regulation was linked to more right-lateralized PFC activity [20, 21, 81, 82]. In our meta-analysis, most of the up-regulation research has focused on rDLPFC [36, 69, 70, 72], because the primary purpose of these research was not to specifically investigate the effect of TMS and tDCS on up-regulation but rather incidental. Therefore, further research is needed to explore the potential effect of TMS and tDCS targeting the left PFC on up-regulation. In addition, the direct comparison between down- and up-regulation goals of explicit ER suggested no significant differences. Overall, these findings highlight the potential benefits of TMS and tDCS -PFC in improving physiological response of down-regulation, while the evidence for its positive effects in subjective experience of explicit ER (down- and up-regulation) is limited.

Evidence from functional imaging studies has revealed that explicit and implicit ER rely on distinct neurocircuits. During explicit ER, the DLPFC and VLPFC modulate the activity in the lateral amygdala subdivision to block the perceptual and semantic inputs [83], whereas during implicit ER, the VMPFC suppresses the activity in the central amygdala subdivision to inhibit the expression or output of emotional response [16, 84]. By perturbing the two prefrontal-subcortical circuits, it is expected that TMS and tDCS affects both forms of ER, but maybe to a different extent. However, our result did not show a significant effect of TMS and tDCS on implicit ER, and the direct comparison of explicit and implicit ER also supported it. One possible explanation is that the effect of TMS and tDCS is easy to reach superficial regions like LPFC, while accessing deeper cortical areas like VMPFC may pose a challenge [85, 86]. Although researchers have utilized the functional connectivity of DLPFC-VMPFC to indirectly modulate the activity of VMPFC by targeting DLPFC [79, 87], functional imaging study had failed to detect a direct connection between the target region and VMPFC [88]. Therefore, standard TMS and tDCS may not effectively stimulate VMPFC to regulate implicit ER. Novel brain stimulation methods, such as high-definition tDCS (HD-tDCS) and deep TMS, enable targeting deep cortical structures, including VMPFC [89, 90]. Evidence demonstrated that HD-tDCS and deep TMS targeting the VMPFC could effectively modulate aggressive responses, social feedback, and theory of mind [91,92,93]. Future research should assess the potential effect of HD-tDCS and deep TMS targeting VMPFC for implicit ER.

Overall, our research distinguishes the different effects of TMS and tDCS on explicit and implicit ER, which indicates that future research needs to tailor TMS and tDCS protocols for explicit and implicit ER.

Factors moderating the effect of excitatory TMS and tDCS on subjective experience of down-regulation

Owing to a large number (n = 19) of the eligible studies, we performed moderation analyses for the subjective experience of down-regulation. The result identified factors including stimulation method, target hemisphere, target area, and stimulation timing. Each moderating factor was separately discussed below.

Stimulation method

Our result indicated that the effect size of TMS studies was significantly larger than that of anodal tDCS studies, which aligns with the results of previous study [43]. This finding may be attributed to differences in the electric field and focality of the two techniques. Evidence from imaging and computational modeling have revealed that in comparison to TMS, tDCS is vulnerable to anatomical factors, such as the thickness of skull and cerebrospinal fluid, which may lead to up to 50% of the electric field intensity being affected [94,95,96]. Moreover, in terms of focal stimulation, TMS exhibits higher spatial precision than tDCS, resulting in a more focused stimulation of the target area [22, 26]. Therefore, compared to tDCS, TMS is a more efficient and promising tool to improve down-regulation function.

Target area and target hemisphere

Our result indicated that the effect size of targeting the rVLPFC is significantly greater than that of lDLPFC, lVLPFC, and rDLPFC. This finding indicated that rVLPFC was the golden target to stimulate to obtain effects on down-regulation. Neuroimage meta-analysis and lesion studies have shown that the VLPFC plays a critical role in down-regulation [14, 15, 97], particularly rVLPFC [98, 99]. Moreover, rVLPFC is also a critical region for inhibition [100, 101]. During down-regulation of emotion, rVLPFC involved the inhibition of negative emotion [20, 99, 102]. A recent TMS study provides causal evidence that further supports the inhibitory role of rVLPFC during down-regulation [103]. Therefore, TMS and tDCS targeting the right VLPFC can produce a larger effect on down-regulation. In addition, we also identified the hemispheric asymmetry in the TMS and tDCS effect on down-regulation, with studies targeting right PFC exhibiting significantly larger effect sizes than those targeting the left PFC. One possible explanation for this finding would be that negative emotions are more closely associated with the right PFC [104,105,106]. In summary, the present findings suggest that right PFC, especially rVLPFC, may be an optimal site for potential intervention in down-regulation.

Stimulation timing

Results suggested that offline TMS and tDCS produced larger effect sizes than online TMS and tDCS. However, it should be noted that all included offline studies were TMS studies, and most online studies were tDCS studies. Cautions should be taken as we cannot rule out the possibility that the modulation effect comes from stimulation method because the effect of TMS is better than tDCS (see the result of stimulation method).

Clinical implications

Deficits in ER are recognized as a core feature of various psychiatric conditions, including major depressive disorder [107, 108], anxiety disorders [109], and autism spectrum disorder [110]. The current findings may hold clinical implications for developing targeted neuromodulation treatments for these disorders. The divergent effects of TMS and tDCS on explicit versus implicit ER suggest protocols could be tailored based on the specific regulation impairments exhibited in a given patient population. Numerous studies, for example, have shown that depressed individuals exhibit compromised explicit ER function [3, 111,112,113], while their implicit ER function remains unaffected. therapeutic approaches for depressed individuals should be tailored to target the neural circuit of explicit ER. It is also notable that a single session of TMS and tDCS in healthy individuals yielded only small effects in this meta-analysis (Hedges' g = − 0.20), which is consistent with previous emotion and er meta-analysis studies [42, 43]. Achieving durable clinical improvements requires repeated TMS and tDCS sessions over weeks to induce synaptic plasticity [114, 115]. Rigorously testing the efficacy and safety of such multi-session protocols in clinical populations will be an important direction before translational application.

Limitations

Several limitations should be put forward. First, although our result demonstrated a positive result on physiological response of down-regulation (3 studies) as well as a null result on implicit ER (5 studies), these findings were not strong enough because of the small number of studies. Further research is required to validate these results. Second, studies included in our meta-analysis mainly used cognitive reappraisal and fear extinction task, only three studies used other tasks [39, 72, 116]. Such high homogeneity makes it difficult to test whether the TMS and tDCS effect varies from different explicit and implicit ER tasks, such as distraction and emotional Stroop task. Therefore, a more diverse explicit and implicit task should be adopted to investigate the TMS and tDCS on ER. Third, besides PFC, other regions including the temporoparietal junction [117], and cerebellum [118] also play an important role in ER. Future meta-analysis could broaden the target area of TMS and tDCS. Fourth, studies included in our meta-analysis primarily utilized the 10–20 EEG system for target localization. While this method is widely accepted, it may present limitations regarding targeting accuracy due to interindividual differences in head shape and cortical anatomy [119, 120]. On the other hand, neuronavigation, which matches MRI-based 3D models of an individual's head and brain images with the actual subject's head [121], offers an individually tailored approach for optimal target localization. While we recognize the potential benefits of neuronavigation, due to its limited usage in the studies we reviewed (only one employed neuronavigation), we could not perform a sub-analysis to determine if this method would yield superior results. Accordingly, we urge future research to explore the use of neuronavigation for more precise localization.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we provided physiological evidence that excitatory TMS and tDCS promotes down-regulation function and identified distinct TMS and tDCS effects on explicit and implicit ER, i.e., TMS and tDCS promotes only explicit but not implicit ER. This distinction between explicit and implicit ER highlights the importance of developing TMS and tDCS protocols that are tailored to different forms of ER. In addition, our moderation analysis indicates a protocol that may achieve an optimal TMS and tDCS effect on down-regulation, i.e., adopting high-frequency rTMS or targeting the rVLPFC, or stimulating in an offline manner. Findings may help refine the scope and usage of the TMS and tDCS protocol, thereby optimizing the effectiveness of TMS and tDCS on ER function.

Availability of data and materials

The data of this study would be available upon reasonable request and with the approval of the corresponding author, Prof. D. Zhang (zhangdd05@gmail.com).

Abbreviations

ER:

Emotion regulation

PFC:

The prefrontal cortex

DLPFC:

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex

VLPFC:

Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex

MPFC:

Medial prefrontal cortex

VMPFC:

Ventral medial prefrontal cortex

tES:

Transcranial electrical stimulation

tDCS:

Transcranial direct current stimulation

tACS:

Transcranial alternating current stimulation

tRNS:

Transcranial random noise stimulation

TMS:

Transcranial magnetic stimulation

rTMS:

Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation

spTMS:

Single pulse TMS

cTBS:

Continuous theta burst stimulation

iTBS:

Intermittent theta burst stimulation

PRISMA:

Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses

CI:

Confident interval

References

  1. Cloitre M, Khan C, Mackintosh MA, Garvert DW, Henn-Haase CM, Falvey EC, et al. Emotion regulation mediates the relationship between ACES and physical and mental health. Psychol Trauma Theory Res Pract Policy. 2019;11(1):82–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0000374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. English T, John OP, Srivastava S, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and peer-rated social functioning: a 4-year longitudinal study. J Res Pers. 2012;46(6):780–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2012.09.006.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;85(2):348–62. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, Drevets WC. A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2008;13(9):833–57. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2008.65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Braunstein LM, Gross JJ, Ochsner KN. Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: a multi-level framework. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12(10):1545–57. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx096.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Gyurak A, Gross JJ, Etkin A. Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: a dual-process framework. Cogn Emot. 2011;25(3):400–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.544160.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Jentsch VL, Wolf OT, Merz CJ. Temporal dynamics of conditioned skin conductance and pupillary responses during fear acquisition and extinction. Int J Psychophysiol. 2020;147:93–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.11.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Kinner VL, Kuchinke L, Dierolf AM, Merz CJ, Otto T, Wolf OT. What our eyes tell us about feelings: tracking pupillary responses during emotion regulation processes. Psychophysiology. 2017;54(4):508–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Lonsdorf TB, Menz MM, Andreatta M, Fullana MA, Golkar A, Haaker J, et al. Don’t fear “fear conditioning”: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;77:247–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Mohammed AR, Kosonogov V, Lyusin D. Expressive suppression versus cognitive reappraisal: effects on self-report and peripheral psychophysiology. Int J Psychophysiol. 2021;167:30–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.06.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Lemaire M, El-Hage W, Frangou S. Reappraising suppression: subjective and physiological correlates of experiential suppression in healthy adults. Front Psychol. 2014;5:571. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00571.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Urry HL. Using reappraisal to regulate unpleasant emotional episodes: goals and timing matter. Emotion. 2009;9(6):782–97. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Goldin PR, Moodie CA, Gross JJ. Acceptance versus reappraisal: behavioral, autonomic, and neural effects. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2019;19(4):927–44. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-019-00690-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, et al. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24(11):2981–90. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M, Laird AR, Fox PT, Habel U. Neural network of cognitive emotion regulation—an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. NeuroImage. 2014;87:345–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Quirk GJ, Garcia R, González-Lima F. Prefrontal mechanisms in extinction of conditioned fear. Biol Psychiat. 2006;60(4):337–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Etkin A, Büchel C, Gross JJ. The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(11):693–700. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4044.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Schiller D, Delgado MR. Overlapping neural systems mediating extinction, reversal and regulation of fear. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(6):268–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Gross JJ, Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations. In: Gross James J, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: The Guilford Press; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Chopra S, Gabrieli JDE, et al. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage. 2004;23(2):483–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Morawetz C, Bode S, Derntl B, Heekeren HR. The effect of strategies, goals and stimulus material on the neural mechanisms of emotion regulation: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;72:111–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Polanía R, Nitsche MA, Ruff CC. Studying and modifying brain function with non-invasive brain stimulation. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21(2):174–87. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-017-0054-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Fertonani A, Miniussi C. Transcranial electrical stimulation: what we know and do not know about mechanisms. Neuroscientist. 2017;23(2):109–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858416631966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Nitsche MA, Paulus W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. 2000;527(3):633–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Nitsche MA, Cohen LG, Wassermann EM, Priori A, Lang N, Antal A, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 2008;1(3):206–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Dayan E, Censor N, Buch ER, Sandrini M, Cohen LG. Noninvasive brain stimulation: from physiology to network dynamics and back. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(7):838–44. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3422.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Kuo M-F, Nitsche MA. Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on cognition. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2012;43(3):192–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1550059412444975.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Hallett M. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a primer. Neuron. 2007;55(2):187–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.026.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Huang Y-Z, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC. Theta burst stimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron. 2005;45(2):201–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Parkin Beth L, Ekhtiari H, Walsh Vincent F. Non-invasive human brain stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: a primer. Neuron. 2015;87(5):932–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.032.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Palmisano A, Bossi F, Barlabà C, Febbraio F, Loconte R, Lupo A, et al. Anodal tDCS effects over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (L-DLPFC) on the rating of facial expression: evidence for a gender-specific effect. Heliyon. 2021;7(11):e08267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e08267.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Vergallito A, Riva P, Pisoni A, Romero Lauro LJ. Modulation of negative emotions through anodal tDCS over the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia. 2018;119:128–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.07.037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Chick CF, Rolle C, Trivedi HM, Monuszko K, Etkin A. Transcranial magnetic stimulation demonstrates a role for the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in emotion perception. Psychiatry Res. 2020;284:112515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Peña-Gómez C, Vidal-Piñeiro D, Clemente IC, Pascual-Leone Á, Bartrés-Faz D. Down-regulation of negative emotional processing by transcranial direct current stimulation: effects of personality characteristics. PloS ONE. 2011;6(7):e22812. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022812.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Cao D, Li Y, Tang Y. Functional specificity of the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in positive reappraisal: a single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2021;21(4):793–804. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-021-00881-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Feeser M, Prehn K, Kazzer P, Mungee A, Bajbouj M. Transcranial direct current stimulation enhances cognitive control during emotion regulation. Brain Stimul. 2014;7(1):105–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2013.08.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. He Z, Zhao J, Shen J, Muhlert N, Elliott R, Zhang D. The right VLPFC and downregulation of social pain: a TMS study. Human Brain Mapp. 2020;41(5):1362–71. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. He Z, Lin Y, Xia L, Liu Z, Zhang D, Elliott R. Critical role of the right VLPFC in emotional regulation of social exclusion: a tDCS study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018;13(4):357–66. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsy026.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Zhao J, Mo L, Bi R, He Z, Chen Y, Xu F, et al. The VLPFC versus the DLPFC in downregulating social pain using reappraisal and distraction strategies. J Neurosci. 2021;41(6):1331–9. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1906-20.2020.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Diefenbach GJ, Assaf M, Goethe JW, Gueorguieva R, Tolin DF. Improvements in emotion regulation following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for generalized anxiety disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 2016;43:1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. He Z, Liu Z, Zhao J, Elliott R, Zhang D. Improving emotion regulation of social exclusion in depression-prone individuals: a tDCS study targeting right VLPFC. Psychol Med. 2020;50(16):2768–79. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002915.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Smits FM, Schutter DJLG, van Honk J, Geuze E. Does non-invasive brain stimulation modulate emotional stress reactivity? Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2020;15(1):23–51. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa011.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Zhang Q, Li X, Liu X, Liu S, Zhang M, Liu Y, et al. The effect of non-invasive brain stimulation on the downregulation of negative emotions: a meta-analysis. Brain Sci. 2022. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12060786.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. The international affective picture system (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention. In: Allen John J.B., editor. Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. Series in affective science. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Shamseer L, Moher D, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ Br Med J. 2015;349:g7647. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.g7647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Higgins JPT, Thomas J, Chandler J, Cumpston M, Li T, Page MJ, Welch VA (editors). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.3 (updated February 2022). Cochrane, 2022. Available from www.training.cochrane.org/handbook.

  47. Choi KM, Scott DT, Lim S-L. The modulating effects of brain stimulation on emotion regulation and decision-making. Neuropsychiatr Electrophysiol. 2016;2(1):4. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40810-016-0018-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Gupta T, Mittal VA. Transcranial direct current stimulation and emotion processing deficits in psychosis and depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2021;271(1):69–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-020-01146-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Lantrip C, Gunning FM, Flashman L, Roth RM, Holtzheimer PE. Effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation on the cognitive control of emotion: potential antidepressant mechanisms. J ECT. 2017;33(2):73–80. https://doi.org/10.1097/yct.0000000000000386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Marković V, Vicario CM, Yavari F, Salehinejad MA, Nitsche MA. A systematic review on the effect of transcranial direct current and magnetic stimulation on fear memory and extinction. Front Human Neurosci. 2021. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2021.655947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Mondino M, Thiffault F, Fecteau S. Does non-invasive brain stimulation applied over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex non-specifically influence mood and emotional processing in healthy individuals? Front Cell Neurosci. 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00399.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Cirillo G, Di Pino G, Capone F, Ranieri F, Florio L, Todisco V, et al. Neurobiological after-effects of non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. 2017;10(1):1–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.11.009.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Duecker F, Sack AT. Rethinking the role of sham TMS. Front Psychol. 2015. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00210.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Dissanayaka TD, Zoghi M, Farrell M, Egan GF, Jaberzadeh S. Sham transcranial electrical stimulation and its effects on corticospinal excitability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Rev Neurosci. 2018;29(2):223–32. https://doi.org/10.1515/revneuro-2017-0026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Drevon D, Fursa SR, Malcolm AL. Intercoder reliability and validity of webplotdigitizer in extracting graphed data. Behav Modif. 2017;41(2):323–39. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445516673998.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Rohatgi AJUhaiW. Webplotdigitizer: Version 4.4. 2020;411. https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/. Accessed Sept 2022.

  57. Borenstein M. Comprehensive meta-analysis software. In: Egger Matthias, Higgins Julian P.T., Smith George Davey, editors. Systematic reviews in health research. Hoboken: Wiley; 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Cambridge: Academic Press; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons; 2021.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  61. Egger M, Smith GD, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Sterne JAC, Sutton AJ, Ioannidis JPA, Terrin N, Jones DR, Lau J, et al. Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials. BMJ. 2011;343:d4002. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d4002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Higgins JP, Sterne JA, Savovic J, Page MJ, Hróbjartsson A, Boutron I, et al. A revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomized trials. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2016;10(Suppl 1):29–31.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Doerig N, Seinsche RJ, Moisa M, Seifritz E, Ruff CC, Kleim B. Enhancing reappraisal of negative emotional memories with transcranial direct current stimulation. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):14760. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-93647-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Chrysikou EG, Wing EK, van Dam WO. Transcranial direct current stimulation over the prefrontal cortex in depression modulates cortical excitability in emotion regulation regions as measured by concurrent functional magnetic resonance imaging: an exploratory study. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;7(1):85–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.12.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Clarke PJF, Haridas SMP, Van Bockstaele B, Chen NTM, Salemink E, Notebaert L. Frontal tDCS and emotional reactivity to negative content: examining the roles of biased interpretation and emotion regulation. Cogn Ther Res. 2021;45(1):19–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-020-10162-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Clarke PJF, Van Bockstaele B, Marinovic W, Howell JA, Boyes ME, Notebaert L. The effects of left DLPFC tDCS on emotion regulation, biased attention, and emotional reactivity to negative content. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2020;20(6):1323–35. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00840-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Hansenne M, Weets E. Anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the left DLPFC improves emotion regulation. Polish Psychol Bulletin. 2020. https://doi.org/10.2442/ppb.2020.132653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Hofhansel L, Habel U, Regenbogen C, Weidler C, Clemens B, Raine A. Stimulating the criminal brain: different effects of prefrontal tDCS in criminal offenders and controls. Brain Stimul. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2020.03.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Marques LM, Morello LYN, Boggio PS. Ventrolateral but not dorsolateral prefrontal cortex tDCS effectively impact emotion reappraisal—effects on emotional experience and interbeat interval. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):15295. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-33711-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. van Dam WO, Chrysikou EG. Effects of unilateral tDCS over left prefrontal cortex on emotion regulation in depression: evidence from concurrent functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2021;21(1):14–34. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00830-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  72. Tu Y, Wilson G, Camprodon J, Dougherty DD, Vangel M, Benedetti F, et al. Manipulating placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia by changing brain excitability. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2021;118(19):e2101273118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2101273118.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Fink J, Exner C. Does transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) improve disgust regulation through imagery rescripting? Front Human Neurosci. 2019. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Vieira L, Marques D, Melo L, Marques RC, Monte-Silva K, Cantilino A. Transcranial direct current stimulation effects on cognitive reappraisal: an unexpected result? Brain Stimul. 2020;13(3):650–2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2020.02.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Jansen JM, van den Heuvel OA, van der Werf YD, de Wit SJ, Veltman DJ, van den Brink W, et al. The effect of high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on emotion processing, reappraisal, and craving in alcohol use disorder patients and healthy controls: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Front Psychiatry. 2019. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00272.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Li S, Xie H, Zheng Z, Chen W, Xu F, Hu X, et al. The causal role of the bilateral ventrolateral prefrontal cortices on emotion regulation of social feedback. Human Brain Mapp. 2022;43(9):2898–910. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25824.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Guhn A, Dresler T, Andreatta M, Müller L, Hahn T, Tupak S, et al. Medial prefrontal cortex stimulation modulates the processing of conditioned fear. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00044.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Dittert N, Hüttner S, Polak T, Herrmann MJ. Augmentation of fear extinction by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Front Behav Neurosci. 2018. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00076.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Deng J, Fang W, Gong Y, Bao Y, Li H, Su S, et al. Augmentation of fear extinction by theta-burst transcranial magnetic stimulation of the prefrontal cortex in humans. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2021;46(2):E292-e302. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.200053.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. van Wout M, Mariano TY, Garnaat SL, Reddy MK, Rasmussen SA, Greenberg BD. Can transcranial direct current stimulation augment extinction of conditioned fear? Brain Stimul. 2016;9(4):529–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Kim SH, Hamann S. Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(5):776–98. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.776%JJournalofCognitiveNeuroscience.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Veit R, Singh V, Sitaram R, Caria A, Rauss K, Birbaumer N. Using real-time fMRI to learn voluntary regulation of the anterior insula in the presence of threat-related stimuli. Soc Cognit Affect Neurosci. 2011;7(6):623–34. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Ochsner KN, Silvers JA, Buhle JT. Functional imaging studies of emotion regulation: a synthetic review and evolving model of the cognitive control of emotion. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2012;1251:E1-24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06751.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Myers KM, Davis M. Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol Psychiatry. 2007;12(2):120–50. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4001939.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Klomjai W, Katz R, Lackmy-Vallée A. Basic principles of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and repetitive TMS (rTMS). Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2015;58(4):208–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rehab.2015.05.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Miranda PC, Lomarev M, Hallett M. Modeling the current distribution during transcranial direct current stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006;117(7):1623–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2006.04.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Ganho-Ávila A, Gonçalves ÓF, Guiomar R, Boggio PS, Asthana MK, Krypotos AM, et al. The effect of cathodal tDCS on fear extinction: a cross-measures study. PloS ONE. 2019;14(9):e0221282. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221282.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Raij T, Nummenmaa A, Marin MF, Porter D, Furtak S, Setsompop K, et al. Prefrontal cortex stimulation enhances fear extinction memory in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;84(2):129–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Kuo HI, Bikson M, Datta A, Minhas P, Paulus W, Kuo MF, et al. Comparing cortical plasticity induced by conventional and high-definition 4 × 1 ring tDCS: a neurophysiological study. Brain Stimul. 2013;6(4):644–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2012.09.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Tendler A, Barnea Ygael N, Roth Y, Zangen A. Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation (dTMS)—beyond depression. Expert Rev Med Devices. 2016;13(10):987–1000. https://doi.org/10.1080/17434440.2016.1233812.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Krause L, Enticott PG, Zangen A, Fitzgerald PB. The role of medial prefrontal cortex in theory of mind: a deep rTMS study. Behav Brain Res. 2012;228(1):87–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Sergiou CS, Santarnecchi E, Romanella SM, Wieser MJ, Franken IHA, Rassin EGC, et al. transcranial direct current stimulation targeting the ventromedial prefrontal cortex reduces reactive aggression and modulates electrophysiological responses in a forensic population. Biol Psychiatry. 2022;7(1):95–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.05.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Zhang D, Ao X, Zheng Z, Shen J, Zhang Y, Gu R. Modulating social feedback processing by deep TMS targeting the medial prefrontal cortex: behavioral and electrophysiological manifestations. NeuroImage. 2022;250:118967. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.118967.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Kim J-H, Kim D-W, Chang WH, Kim Y-H, Kim K, Im C-H. Inconsistent outcomes of transcranial direct current stimulation may originate from anatomical differences among individuals: electric field simulation using individual MRI data. Neurosci Lett. 2014;564:6–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2014.01.054.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Miranda PC, Mekonnen A, Salvador R, Ruffini G. The electric field in the cortex during transcranial current stimulation. NeuroImage. 2013;70:48–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Opitz A, Paulus W, Will S, Antunes A, Thielscher A. Determinants of the electric field during transcranial direct current stimulation. NeuroImage. 2015;109:140–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Salas CE, Gross JJ, Rafal RD, Viñas-Guasch N, Turnbull OH. Concrete behaviour and reappraisal deficits after a left frontal stroke: a case study. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2013;23(4):467–500. https://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2013.784709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Berkman ET, Lieberman MD. Using neuroscience to broaden emotion regulation: theoretical and methodological considerations. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2009;3(4):475–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00186.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  99. Wager TD, Davidson ML, Hughes BL, Lindquist MA, Ochsner KN. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron. 2008;59(6):1037–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(4):170–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  101. Tabibnia G, Monterosso JR, Baicy K, Aron AR, Poldrack RA, Chakrapani S, et al. Different forms of self-control share a neurocognitive substrate. J Neurosci. 2011;31(13):4805–10. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2859-10.2011.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Lee T-W, Dolan RJ, Critchley HD. Controlling emotional expression: behavioral and neural correlates of nonimitative emotional responses. Cereb Cortex. 2007;18(1):104–13. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Cheng S, Qiu X, Li S, Mo L, Xu F, Zhang D. Different roles of the left and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in cognitive reappraisal: an online transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Front Human Neurosci. 2022. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.928077.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Goodman RN, Rietschel JC, Lo L-C, Costanzo ME, Hatfield BD. Stress, emotion regulation and cognitive performance: the predictive contributions of trait and state relative frontal EEG alpha asymmetry. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;87(2):115–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.09.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Harmon-Jones E, Allen JJ. Anger and frontal brain activity: EEG asymmetry consistent with approach motivation despite negative affective valence. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;74(5):1310–6. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.5.1310.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  106. Wheeler RE, Davidson RJ, Tomarken AJ. Frontal brain asymmetry and emotional reactivity: a biological substrate of affective style. Psychophysiology. 1993;30(1):82–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03207.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  107. Joormann J, Stanton CH. Examining emotion regulation in depression: a review and future directions. Behav Res Ther. 2016;86:35–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.07.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Joormann J, Quinn ME. Cognitive processes and emotion regulation in depression. Depression Anxiety. 2014;31(4):308–15. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Amstadter A. Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. J Anxiety Disord. 2008;22(2):211–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Cai RY, Richdale AL, Uljarević M, Dissanayake C, Samson AC. Emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder: where we are and where we need to go. Autism Res. 2018;11(7):962–78. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1968.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  111. Campbell-Sills L, Barlow DH, Brown TA, Hofmann SG. Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(9):1251–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2005.10.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  112. Garnefski N, Rieffe C, Jellesma F, Terwogt MM, Kraaij V. Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and emotional problems in 9–11-year-old children: the development of an instrument. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;16(1):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-006-0562-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  113. Garnefski N, Kraaij VJP. Relationships between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms: a comparative study of five specific samples. Pers Individ Differ. 2006;40(8):1659–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Brunoni AR, Nitsche MA, Bolognini N, Bikson M, Wagner T, Merabet L, et al. Clinical research with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): challenges and future directions. Brain Stimul. 2012;5(3):175–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2011.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  115. Lefaucheur JP, Aleman A, Baeken C, Benninger DH, Brunelin J, Di Lazzaro V, et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): an update (2014–2018). Clin Neurophysiol. 2020;131(2):474–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2019.11.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  116. Urgesi C, Mattiassi ADA, Buiatti T, Marini A. Tell it to a child! a brain stimulation study of the role of left inferior frontal gyrus in emotion regulation during storytelling. NeuroImage. 2016;136:26–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.039.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  117. Powers JP, Davis SW, Neacsiu AD, Beynel L, Appelbaum LG, LaBar KS. Examining the role of lateral parietal cortex in emotional distancing using TMS. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2020;20(5):1090–102. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-020-00821-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  118. Schutter DJLG, van Honk J. The cerebellum in emotion regulation: a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Cerebellum. 2009;8(1):28–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12311-008-0056-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  119. Uylings HB, Rajkowska G, Sanz-Arigita E, Amunts K, Zilles K. Consequences of large interindividual variability for human brain atlases: converging macroscopical imaging and microscopical neuroanatomy. Anat Embryol. 2005;210(5–6):423–31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-005-0042-4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  120. Herwig U, Satrapi P, Schönfeldt-Lecuona C. Using the international 10–20 EEG system for positioning of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Brain Topogr. 2003;16(2):95–9. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:brat.0000006333.93597.9d.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  121. Lioumis P, Rosanova M. The role of neuronavigation in TMS-EEG studies: current applications and future perspectives. J Neurosci Methods. 2022;380:109677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2022.109677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32100855; 32271102), the Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science (2022SHIBS0003) and the Shenzhen Science and Technology Program (20220810124032001).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

XQ, ZH, and XC collected and prepared data for meta-analysis. XQ analyzed data. XQ, ZH, and DZ wrote the paper. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dandan Zhang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Characteristics of inhibitory TMS of explicit and implicit ER studies. Table S2. Characteristics of cathode tDCS of explicit and implicit ER studies. Table S3. The effect of excitatory and inhibitory TMS and tDCS on the no-regulation condition of explicit ER. Table S4. The effect of excitatory and inhibitory TMS and tDCS on the no-regulation condition of implicit ER. Figure S1. Risk of bias summary of all included studies (n = 6). (A) Methodological quality assessment of each study at 6 domains was illustrated. (B) Risk of bias graph. Figure S2. Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of inhibitory TMS and tDCS on the subjective experience of down-regulation. Combied: Studies with multiple outcomes within a study were combined into averaged data with CMA, which can prevent an improper estimate of the precision of the summary effect. Figure S3. Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of inhibitory TMS and tDCS on subjective experience of up-regulation. Figure S4. Forest plot for the summary effect size on the effect of inhibitory TMS and tDCS on subjective experience of implicit ER. Combied: Studies with multiple outcomes (e.g. valence and arousal) within a study were combined into averaged data with CMA, which can prevent an improper estimate of the precision of the summary effect. Figure S5. Sensitivity analysis with one study removed for the excitatory TMS and tDCS effect on the subjective experience of down-regulation. Figure S6. Sensitivity analysis with one study removed for the excitatory TMS and tDCS effect on the physiological response of down-regulation. Figure S7. Sensitivity analysis with one study removed for the excitatory TMS and tDCS effect on the subjective experience of up-regulation. Figure S8. Sensitivity analysis with one study removed for the excitatory TMS and tDCS effect on the physiological response of implicit ER.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Qiu, X., He, Z., Cao, X. et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and transcranial direct current stimulation affect explicit but not implicit emotion regulation: a meta-analysis. Behav Brain Funct 19, 15 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-023-00217-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-023-00217-8

Keywords